2007
DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.12.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Let’s you do that: Sharing the cognitive burdens of dialogue

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
24
0

Year Published

2007
2007
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
0
24
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Often resources are provided for joint actors on the assumption that whatever is available to an actor is used. Sometimes (see Bard, Anderson, et al, 2007) this proves not to be the case. The data that were recorded for the present experiment permitted very fine-grained analyses of action/gaze sequences that would allow us to discover without further coding who consults what information at critical phases of their actions.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Often resources are provided for joint actors on the assumption that whatever is available to an actor is used. Sometimes (see Bard, Anderson, et al, 2007) this proves not to be the case. The data that were recorded for the present experiment permitted very fine-grained analyses of action/gaze sequences that would allow us to discover without further coding who consults what information at critical phases of their actions.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The effect of copresence on joint action (Fussell & Kraut, 2004;Horton & Keysar, 1996;Kraut, Fussell, & Siegel, 2003;Kraut et al, 2002) can also easily be manipulated by altering the proximity of the two eyetrackers-the limit only depending on the network connections. Similarly, the effect of perspective on language use (Hanna & Tanenhaus, 2004), including the use of deictic expressions such as "this" or "that," can be investigated, as well as the differences in eye movements during comprehension (Spivey, Tanenhaus, Eberhard, & Sedivy, 2002) versus production (Horton & Keysar, 1996), and whether speakers engage in "audience design" or instead operate along more egocentric lines (Bard, Anderson, et al, 2007;Bell, 1984). Functional roles, such as instruction giver or follower, can be assigned to members of a dyad to determine how this might influence gaze behavior and the formation of referring expressions (Engelhardt, Bailey, & Ferreira, 2006).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Perspective taking on addressee's point of view on mutual awareness; · Higher levels of consideration of each other's mutual beliefs are possible, but are rarely deployed. These strategies are listed in order of ascending complexity [12]. Complexity concerns Human beings, who tend to rely on the simplest sufficient strategy for the current purpose, but concerns also the OSS if these strategies are to be implemented.…”
Section: Understanding and Non-understandingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, when another individual's attention is manifested in a visible eyetrack throughout a shared task, speakers' task strategies differ globally from those used in sessions without this cue (Bard, et al, 2007;Brennan, Chen, Dickinson, Neider, & Zelinsky, 2008). In a route communication task, however, Bard et al (2007) also found that speakers' use of the interlocutor's eyetrack was limited: they recognized that its arrival at the goal of an current instruction signalled its success, but were insensitive to its location elsewhere when it indicated an error, and they failed to make genuinely contingent responses. In a similar task , interlocutors made a behavioural distinction between global availability and local use of one another's faces (Anderson, Bard, Sotillo, Newlands, & Doherty-Sneddon, 1997).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%