2005
DOI: 10.1016/j.specom.2005.02.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Lexical bias revisited: Detecting, rejecting and repairing speech errors in inner speech

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
70
0
4

Year Published

2008
2008
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 52 publications
(75 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
(56 reference statements)
1
70
0
4
Order By: Relevance
“…A common observation in the conversation-analytic literature on self-repair (Schegloff 1979, Fox and Jasperson 1995, Jasperson 2002, Rieger 2003 is that in spontaneous conversation, speakers tend to do self-repair work without delay, while other -repair -in which listeners notice an error and invite the speaker to repair it -is frequently delayed, if not avoided altogether, as shown by Schegloff et al (1977). Psycholinguistic studies confirm that speakers tend to initiate self-repair as soon as an error is detected (Levelt 1983, Blackmer and Mitton 1991, Nooteboom 2005.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…A common observation in the conversation-analytic literature on self-repair (Schegloff 1979, Fox and Jasperson 1995, Jasperson 2002, Rieger 2003 is that in spontaneous conversation, speakers tend to do self-repair work without delay, while other -repair -in which listeners notice an error and invite the speaker to repair it -is frequently delayed, if not avoided altogether, as shown by Schegloff et al (1977). Psycholinguistic studies confirm that speakers tend to initiate self-repair as soon as an error is detected (Levelt 1983, Blackmer and Mitton 1991, Nooteboom 2005.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…All instances involve the overt correction or reformulation of immediately prior speech. Instances of 'covert repair' such as hesitations, repetitions and restarts (Levelt 1983, Postma and Kolk 1993, Fox Tree and Clark 1997, Nooteboom 2005 were not included. It should be noted that the material used for this study is rather different from that used in some of the earlier studies of self-repair referred to above -in particular Levelt (1983) and Levelt and Cutler (1983).…”
Section: Datamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous research has shown that speakers are more likely to produce errors resulting in new words than pseudowords, indicating that lexicality is a filter used to intercept errors during speech production (Baars, Motley, & MacKay, 1975;Hartsuiker, Corley & Martensen, 2005;Nooteboom, 2005). Returning to our task, the following three predictions can be made: 1) Impaired language processing of any sort (including monitoring) would be apparent through slower response times and/or higher error rates; 2) Impaired self-monitoring for accuracy would be indexed by an overall increased error-rate; 3) Impaired selfmonitoring for lexicality would result in a modulation of errors as a function of their lexical status.…”
Section: The Current Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The use of this criterion is supported by the lexical bias effect both in corpus studies (e.g., Hartsuiker, Anton-Mendez, Roelstraete, & Costa, 2006;Nooteboom, 2005b) and in the SLIP task (Spoonerisms of Laboratory Induced Dispositions; e.g., Baars, Motley, & MacKay, 1975;Dell, 1986;Hartsuiker et al, 2006;Humphreys, 2002;Nooteboom, 2005a;Nooteboom & Quené, 2008). For example, in the SLIP task, word pairs that have to be read aloud are preceded by word pairs that bias them into making errors, like exchanging the first phonemes of each word (tool carts -> cool tarts).…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%