1993
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-1770.1993.tb00174.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Lexical Development in Bilingual Infants and Toddlers: Comparison to Monolingual Norms

Abstract: This study compares lexical development in a sample of 25 simultaneous bilingual and 35 monolingual children for whom semilongitudinal data were collected between the ages of 8 and 30 months. A standardized parent report form, the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (1989), was used to assess the children's receptive and productive vocabulary in English and/or Spanish. A methodology was devised to assess the degree of overlap between the bilingual children's lexical knowledge in one language and thei… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

49
642
9
14

Year Published

2002
2002
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 733 publications
(714 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
49
642
9
14
Order By: Relevance
“…Bilinguals have also been found to perform significantly better than monolinguals on measures of task switching (Bialystok & Martin, 2004;Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008). In contrast, on standardized measures of vocabulary young bilinguals frequently perform at lower levels than their monolingual peers, especially if only one of their languages is considered (Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008;Namazi & Thordardottir, 2010;Pearson, Fernández, & Oller, 1995;Thordardottir, Rothenberg, Rivard, & Naves, 2006). One important cognitive mechanism that has been consistently linked to cognitive control is working memory.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Bilinguals have also been found to perform significantly better than monolinguals on measures of task switching (Bialystok & Martin, 2004;Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008). In contrast, on standardized measures of vocabulary young bilinguals frequently perform at lower levels than their monolingual peers, especially if only one of their languages is considered (Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008;Namazi & Thordardottir, 2010;Pearson, Fernández, & Oller, 1995;Thordardottir, Rothenberg, Rivard, & Naves, 2006). One important cognitive mechanism that has been consistently linked to cognitive control is working memory.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, this difference was noted in three studies: the first for infants and toddlers exposed to Spanish and English (Pearson et al, 1993); the second for toddlers exposed to German and English (Junker & Stockman, 2002); and the third for 5-year-olds exposed to English and French (Thordardottir, 2011).…”
Section: Benefits and Challenges Of Learning Two Languages From Birthmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…There is a significant difference in expressive vocabulary for these children when comparing the vocabulary of one of the languages to their monolingual counterparts (Junker & Stockman, 2002;Pearson, Fernández, & Oller, 1993;Thordardottir, 2011). For example, this difference was noted in three studies: the first for infants and toddlers exposed to Spanish and English (Pearson et al, 1993); the second for toddlers exposed to German and English (Junker & Stockman, 2002); and the third for 5-year-olds exposed to English and French (Thordardottir, 2011).…”
Section: Benefits and Challenges Of Learning Two Languages From Birthmentioning
confidence: 98%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Since bilingual children use their two languages in different contexts, a bilingual child may not have as large a vocabulary in each of the languages as an age-matched monolingual child. However, the bilingual child's total vocabulary will usually be larger than that of the monolingual child (Pearson, Fernandez, & Oller, 1993;see Bialystok, 2001, for a review).…”
Section: Consequences Of Becoming a Bilingualmentioning
confidence: 99%