2014
DOI: 10.1167/14.9.13
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Local edge statistics provide information regarding occlusion and nonocclusion edges in natural scenes

Abstract: Edges in natural scenes can result from a number of different causes. In this study, we investigated the statistical differences between edges arising from occlusions and nonocclusions (reflectance differences, surface change, and cast shadows). In the first experiment, edges in natural scenes were identified using the Canny edge detection algorithm. Observers then classified these edges as either an occlusion edge (one region of an image occluding another) or a nonocclusion edge. The nonocclusion edges were f… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

8
34
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
4
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(42 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
8
34
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Computationally, color and texture cues can be useful in picking out important edges (e.g., Martin et al 2004), and contour blur information can signal edges caused by shadows and shading (Elder 1999, Figures 1c and 3). Luminance contrast can be used to discriminate boundary edges from other edges (Vilankar et al 2014), andEhinger et al (2017) have shown that a deep neural network (DNN) can be trained to use local luminance, color, texture, and orientation cues to distinguish depth from nondepth edges. Little is known about the physiological mechanisms underlying the human visual system's ability to distinguish between these different physical edge classes.…”
Section: Edge Detectionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Computationally, color and texture cues can be useful in picking out important edges (e.g., Martin et al 2004), and contour blur information can signal edges caused by shadows and shading (Elder 1999, Figures 1c and 3). Luminance contrast can be used to discriminate boundary edges from other edges (Vilankar et al 2014), andEhinger et al (2017) have shown that a deep neural network (DNN) can be trained to use local luminance, color, texture, and orientation cues to distinguish depth from nondepth edges. Little is known about the physiological mechanisms underlying the human visual system's ability to distinguish between these different physical edge classes.…”
Section: Edge Detectionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This may be a more efficient way to approach the problem, since edges (of any kind) make up only a very small percentage of an image. Previous work on this approach comes from Vilankar et al [17], who investigated depth versus non-depth edge classification by human observers. They found that luminance contrast was a particularly strong cue for this task and predicted human edge classification with 83% accuracy.…”
Section: Prior Workmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A candidate model can be used to predict which images should be metamers. This logic allows the simultaneous experimental testing of a large number of image-based cues (that is, all cues encoded by a given model), complementing approaches to natural scene perception in which at most a handful of cues are manipulated at once (e.g., Alam, Vilankar, Field, & Chandler, 2014;Bex, 2010;Bex, Mareschal, & Dakin, 2007;Bex, Solomon, & Dakin, 2009;Bradley, Abrams, & Geisler, 2014;Dorr & Bex, 2013;Haun & Peli, 2013;McDonald & Tadmor, 2006;Tadmor & Tolhurst, 1994;Thomson, Foster, & Summers, 2000;To, Gilchrist, Troscianko, & Tolhurst, 2011;Vilankar, Golden, Chandler, & Field, 2014;Vilankar, Vasu, & Chandler, 2011; Wallis & Bex, 2012; Wallis, Dorr, & Figure 1. Model predictions and metamerism.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%