2016
DOI: 10.1152/jn.00938.2015
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Locomotor sequence learning in visually guided walking

Abstract: Choi JT, Jensen P, Nielsen JB. Locomotor sequence learning in visually guided walking. J Neurophysiol 115: 2014 -2020, 2016. First published February 10, 2016 doi:10.1152/jn.00938.2015.-Voluntary limb modifications must be integrated with basic walking patterns during visually guided walking. In this study we tested whether voluntary gait modifications can become more automatic with practice. We challenged walking control by presenting visual stepping targets that instructed subjects to modify step length fro… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
18
0
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
3
18
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Our results imply that the ability to perform goal-directed movements with the lower limbs is mature around the age of 8 years. Similarly, Choi et al ( 2016 ) revealed stable, adult-like step accuracy on step targets in children aged 11–16 years (comparable to our sample of 9–18 years). These authors did, however, show decreasing step accuracy in younger children aged 6–10 years.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Our results imply that the ability to perform goal-directed movements with the lower limbs is mature around the age of 8 years. Similarly, Choi et al ( 2016 ) revealed stable, adult-like step accuracy on step targets in children aged 11–16 years (comparable to our sample of 9–18 years). These authors did, however, show decreasing step accuracy in younger children aged 6–10 years.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Studies illustrate an ongoing refinement of complex gait strategies after the age of 8 years during complex gait tasks such as obstacle avoidance, precision stepping, and dual-task walking. This refinement is reflected by improving gait speed and obstacle clearance (Pryde et al 1997 ; Michel et al 2010 ), the presence of adult-like muscle activations (McFadyen et al 2001 ), and more efficient foot placements (Berard and Vallis 2006 ; Choi et al 2016 ; Corporaal et al 2016 ). It has been suggested, but not established, that this extended refinement relates to ongoing neural maturation of cognitive processes underlying the control of complex gait (e.g., Pryde et al 1997 ; Choi et al 2016 ; Corporaal et al 2016 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, other work has shown that visual feedback can drive locomotor learning mechanisms that do not require adaptation or prediction error, such as sequence learning [28]. In our study, visual feedback drove voluntary correction and not adaptation because the split-belt perturbation is mechanical (i.e., the belts move at different speeds) and the prediction error is detected through proprioception.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 73%
“…The treadmill speed was then determined by multiplying the subject's step length with 3.9 in order to match the cadence of 80 steps per minute between subjects (see Choi et al. ()). Prior to the visually guided walking, subjects were carefully introduced to the task.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The step length was defined as 2/3 of the leg length measured from the left greater trochanter to the left lateral malleolus. The treadmill speed was then determined by multiplying the subject's step length with 3.9 in order to match the cadence of 80 steps per minute between subjects (see Choi et al (2016)). Prior to the visually guided walking, subjects were carefully introduced to the task.…”
Section: Experimental Setup and Visual Feedbackmentioning
confidence: 99%