1998
DOI: 10.1007/s11881-998-0005-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Longitudinal course of rapid naming in disabled and nondisabled readers

Abstract: Phoneme awareness is a powerful conceptual achievement-both for the would-be reader and for the scientist seeking to understand the crucial links between spoken and written language. But it has long been recognized by both researchers and practitioners (1) that success in phoneme awareness depends crucially on other more basic factors; and (2) that other factors may also contribute to the reading difficulties of some children. We are pleased to publish this year four papers that consider correlates of reading … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

10
74
1
2

Year Published

2002
2002
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 74 publications
(87 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
10
74
1
2
Order By: Relevance
“…This is consistent with the few previous studies in this area (e.g., Chiappe et al, 2002;Felton et al, 1990). Although adult normative data on the RAN do not exist, it is possible to gain an estimate of the severity of naming speed impairment in the RD sample by using normative data for Grade 8 students (normative data provided in Meyer et al, 1998a;see Note). Compared to eighth-grade students, the control group performed between the 50th and 75th percentiles on RAN, which is expected if the skills used in naming speed plateau in adolescence.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This is consistent with the few previous studies in this area (e.g., Chiappe et al, 2002;Felton et al, 1990). Although adult normative data on the RAN do not exist, it is possible to gain an estimate of the severity of naming speed impairment in the RD sample by using normative data for Grade 8 students (normative data provided in Meyer et al, 1998a;see Note). Compared to eighth-grade students, the control group performed between the 50th and 75th percentiles on RAN, which is expected if the skills used in naming speed plateau in adolescence.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…The four RAN tasks were averaged to produce a RAN composite measure (Meyer et al, 1998a). The control group was significantly faster than the RD group on this composite, t(51) = 4.65, p < .0001, d = 1.30.…”
Section: Persistence Of Phonological and Naming Speed Deficitsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This cutoff level is similar to that used by other researchers in the field of reading disabilities (e.g., Hurford, Schauf, Bunce, Blaich, & Moore, 1994;Meyer, Wood, Hart, & Felton, 1998). It also represents a compromise criterion level when compared to that found in more liberal definitions (25th percentile; Fletcher et al, 1994;Stanovich & Siegel, 1994) or in more conservative definitions of reading disabilities (1.5 SD; Badian, McAnulty, Duffy, & Als, 1990).…”
Section: Participantssupporting
confidence: 64%
“…As mentioned above, the letter and digit forms of the RAN (particularly the letter form, see Neuhaus, Foorman, Francis & Carlson, 2001) are good and robust predictors of single word reading, though not always of reading comprehension (Meyer, Wood, Hart, & Felton, 1998;Scarborough & Domgaard, 1998). While the rapid naming of objects and colours do not consistently predict reading ability (Blachman, 1984;Denckla, & Rudel, 1974;Denckla & Rudel, 1976a;Manis & Doi, 1995;Wimmer, 1993;Wolf, Bally, & Morris, 1986), RAN tasks, particularly RANL and RAND, would seem to be specific predictors of reading problems.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%