2013
DOI: 10.1111/pops.12012
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Loss Aversion and Foreign Policy Resolve

Abstract: This article draws upon recent findings from the field of neuroscience to explore how loss aversion affects foreign policy resolve. We theorize that U.S. policy makers are more resolute in pursuing preventive policies that seek to avoid losses than they are in pursuing promotive policies that seek to acquire new gains. To test our theory, we conduct the first large‐n analysis of foreign policy hypotheses derived from the neuroscience of loss aversion using data from 100 cases of U.S.‐initiated Section 301 trad… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

1
16
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 78 publications
1
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…189-199) has been found to explain a wide variety of apparently ''irrational'' choices and behaviors, in different contexts. An interesting recent application is Berejikian and Early (2013), who use the endowment effect to explain ''foreign policy resolve,'' or the willingness of nations to expend substantial resources to win back concessions, territory, or favorable policies that are perceived as belonging to the nation. They seem much less willing to expend resources or take risks to obtain equivalent new benefits of comparable or even larger size.…”
Section: Loss Aversion/endowment Effectsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…189-199) has been found to explain a wide variety of apparently ''irrational'' choices and behaviors, in different contexts. An interesting recent application is Berejikian and Early (2013), who use the endowment effect to explain ''foreign policy resolve,'' or the willingness of nations to expend substantial resources to win back concessions, territory, or favorable policies that are perceived as belonging to the nation. They seem much less willing to expend resources or take risks to obtain equivalent new benefits of comparable or even larger size.…”
Section: Loss Aversion/endowment Effectsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the domain of international conflict, risk seeking in losses principally manifests in four ways. First, decision making elites are likely to advocate riskier (and perhaps more conflictual) foreign policies to preserve their state's reputation or position in international politics than they would advocate to improve that reputation or position (Berejikian and Early 2013;Jervis 1992: 192-193;Levy 2000Levy : 200-210, 2003; see also Renshon 2015). Second, if a violent conflict has begun, decision makers are likely to view sunk costs like casualties "as a certain loss in the absence of further action" (Levy 2000: 204; see also Renshon 2015) and therefore persevere to make sure the casualties have not been in vain.…”
Section: Errors and Biasesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The perceptions that resolve in negotiations and assertive or aggressive acts can cow an opponent serve to buttress hawkish decision makers' predilection for intransigence and forcefulness in foreign policy.While the other biases and errors I have discussed serve mainly to foster hawkish decision makers' inflexibility in negotiations, the risk seeking in losses bias encourages warhawks, hawks, and chickenhawks to take forceful foreign policy positions. Specifically, this bias makes hawkish elites likely to advocate risky conflictual foreign policy action to avoid granting concessions to an adversary or otherwise incurring certain losses at the hands of an opponent (seeBerejikian and Early 2013;Farnham 1992;Jervis 1992;…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Individuals with more knowledge and investment experience tend to be less loss averse. This means that loss aversion can be mitigated with experience (Berejikian & Early, 2013;Bokhari & Geltner, 2011).…”
Section: Loss Aversionmentioning
confidence: 99%