1965
DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8317.1965.tb00339.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION OF HEAVINESS AND LOUDNESS BY INDIVIDUAL SUBJECTS: A TEST OF A PROBABILISTIC RESPONSE THEORY1

Abstract: One hundred magnitude estimates of heaviness were obtained for each of 18 weights from each of six S s and of loudness for each of 20 intensities from another six Ss. T h e stimuli were spaced at approximately equal logarithmic intervals. T h e mean magnitude functions of individual Ss appear, in general, to deviate systematically from power functions. The distributions of responses, normalized by the mean response, appear to be skewed, highly peaked, and to have high tails. For at least half of the Ss, this r… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
45
0

Year Published

1970
1970
2009
2009

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 114 publications
(49 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
4
45
0
Order By: Relevance
“…I. As is by now familiar from a number of studies (Braida & Durlach, 1972;Luce & Mo, 1965;Pradhan & Hoffman, 1963), functions obtained with this many observations per point are roughly power functions, but with deviations of as much as 5 dB from the best fitting power functions. (This is equally true when only a few observations are obtained, but the variability of one or two responses per signal is so great that one cannot reject the hypothesis of a power function; see Ekman et al, 1967, andGuirao, 1964.)…”
Section: Experiments 2 Magnitude Estimation Without a Standardmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…I. As is by now familiar from a number of studies (Braida & Durlach, 1972;Luce & Mo, 1965;Pradhan & Hoffman, 1963), functions obtained with this many observations per point are roughly power functions, but with deviations of as much as 5 dB from the best fitting power functions. (This is equally true when only a few observations are obtained, but the variability of one or two responses per signal is so great that one cannot reject the hypothesis of a power function; see Ekman et al, 1967, andGuirao, 1964.)…”
Section: Experiments 2 Magnitude Estimation Without a Standardmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…This variability is both between observers, with exponents of the power function fitted to loudness data ranging from at least .15 to .60 (using an intensity measure), and within observers, with the coefficient of variation (aim) being of the order of .2-.3 (Luce & Mo, 1965;Schneider & Lane, 1963;Stevens & Guirao, 1964). The question we wish to consider here is whether this variability contains any interesting information.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It also appears desIJite variatien between experiments and between Ss within experiments in the range of numerical estimates used (cf. Luce & Mo, 1965 The standard stimulus, presented once only at the beginning of the experiment, was the 5()().g weight. The S then judged all 17 weights for JO trials.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The Lognormal Assumption The data of Luce and Mo (1965) suggest that the distribution of magnitude estimates may not he lognormal. This may well be correct.…”
Section: Substituting These Values In Equation 16mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Edwards (1968, p. 78) illustrates how the sampling distribution of the mean of even a completely rectangular distribution approaches normality closely in samples as small as n = 4. For unimodal leptokurtic distributions like those in Figure 6 of Luce and Mo (1965), the approach to normality should be even more rapid. To check on this assumption, we constructed hypothetical distributions similar to those in Figure 6 of Luce and Mo, and the actual standard errors of the geometric means in samples of size n = 3 were determined using Monte Carlo methods.…”
Section: Substituting These Values In Equation 16mentioning
confidence: 99%