2005
DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.08.022
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Manipulability and living/non-living category effects on object identification

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
24
0
2

Year Published

2006
2006
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 46 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
1
24
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Such a difference was not found with visual pictures since the peak of the LPC for living items was not significantly earlier than that detected for than non-living items. However, these results are consistent with our own reaction time data which demonstrate that living items are selected earlier than non-living items and with the results of other studies in which living items were identified prior to non-living items in both modalities in a picture-word matching task e.g., (Fuggetta et al, 2009) when familiarity, manipulability and level of identification are controlled for (Filliter et al, 2005). …”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Such a difference was not found with visual pictures since the peak of the LPC for living items was not significantly earlier than that detected for than non-living items. However, these results are consistent with our own reaction time data which demonstrate that living items are selected earlier than non-living items and with the results of other studies in which living items were identified prior to non-living items in both modalities in a picture-word matching task e.g., (Fuggetta et al, 2009) when familiarity, manipulability and level of identification are controlled for (Filliter et al, 2005). …”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…On most accounts of semantic memory, these action features of objects are represented separately from other kinds of semantic features, like color, shape, and typical location (e.g., Allport, 1985; Warrington and McCarthy, 1987; McRae et al, 1997; Barsalou, 1999). Furthermore, the presence of action features for some objects may drive the broad differentiation between manipulable and non-manipulable objects that is observed both in behavioral (e.g., Filliter et al, 2005; Siakaluk et al, 2008) and neuroimaging (see Beauchamp and Martin, 2007 for a review) studies. Thus, for manipulable artifacts, thematic relationships between objects related by virtue of a common action (e.g., hammer/nail) may be more salient than relationships between objects that merely occur within the same event without directly interacting (e.g., hammer and screw co-occur in a “carpentry” event).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, manipulability could be a confounded factor when comparing natural (e.g., animals) and artifact (e.g., tools or household items) concepts. Filliter, McMullen, and Westwood (2005) demonstrated that when object domain and object manipulability were specifically dissociated, object identification speed depended on object manipulability. Thus, although object manipulability appears to be a decisive factor in adult conceptual processing, the distinction between manipulable and nonmanipulable objects has never been explored in research on conceptual development.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%