2012
DOI: 10.1177/1367006912438991
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Markedness, input frequency, and the acquisition of inflection: Evidence from Basque/Spanish bilingual children

Abstract: Crosslinguistically, children begin producing the person and number features of personal pronouns in a similar order. This article explored whether the same is true of verbal agreement morphology and evaluated three potential explanatory hypotheses which could account for a universal sequence of the development of phi features: the existence of an innate feature geometry, statistical properties in the input, and the organization of verbal paradigms. I examined these hypotheses in light of data from 20 bilingua… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
5
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
1
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Secondly, both groups showed a phonological effect whereby ergative -k is more likely to be produced in prevocalic contexts. This finding is consistent with the Stop Deletion Rule in Basque (Hualde, 1991) and reported in production studies (Austin, 2013;Ezeizabarrena & Larrañaga, 1996;Rodríguez-Ordóñez, 2015. Third, whereas personal pronouns showed a general trend in favoring ergative case marking, first-and third-person subjects also showed a disfavoring effect.…”
Section: Linguistic Stability Despite Differences In Constraintssupporting
confidence: 90%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Secondly, both groups showed a phonological effect whereby ergative -k is more likely to be produced in prevocalic contexts. This finding is consistent with the Stop Deletion Rule in Basque (Hualde, 1991) and reported in production studies (Austin, 2013;Ezeizabarrena & Larrañaga, 1996;Rodríguez-Ordóñez, 2015. Third, whereas personal pronouns showed a general trend in favoring ergative case marking, first-and third-person subjects also showed a disfavoring effect.…”
Section: Linguistic Stability Despite Differences In Constraintssupporting
confidence: 90%
“…(1) The variation in Basque ergativity has been extensively examined in syntactic theory (Preminger, 2012;Rezac et al, 2014), historical linguistics (Aldai, 2009), child language acquisition (Austin, 2007(Austin, , 2013Ezeizabarrena & Larrañaga, 1996), and psycholinguistics (Díaz et al, 2016;Zawiszewski et al, 2011). In particular, the syntactic status of unergative verbs (Bobaljik, 1993;Hale & Keyser, 1993) shows a clear dialectal divide (see Berro & Etxepare, 2017;Pineda & Berro, 2020 for recent discussions).…”
Section: Verb Typementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In the nominal inflection, however, ergativity is developmental: it first emerges around the age of 2;01 and continues to be very variable until children are around 3;00, consistent with other ergative languages (Bavin & Stoll, 2013; Pye et al, 2013). This alternation between ergative –k and zero-marking has been termed Optional Ergative Case Stage with evidence pointing that such a stage is more drawn-out in bilingual children (Austin, 2007, 2013). Two important input-related factors have been claimed to contribute in this optional stage.…”
Section: Basque Ergativitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…6 Austin (2013) also observes that bilingual Spanish-Basque children produce 3rd person more frequently than 1st person, as do the adults that surround them. An anonymous reviewer notes that an overwhelming number of experimental items would be considered ungrammatical in monolingual Spanish.…”
Section: Data Availability Statementmentioning
confidence: 88%