2008
DOI: 10.1007/s10869-008-9064-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Middle Response Functioning in Likert-responses to Personality Items

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
100
0
7

Year Published

2011
2011
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 135 publications
(109 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
2
100
0
7
Order By: Relevance
“…Additionally, many studies question central values in response scales given that the verbal statement that acompanies the central number does not mean the individual considers they are in the middle of the continuum of the corresponding latent construct (Dalal et al 2014;González-Romá and Espejo 2003;Hernández et al Kulas et al 2008;Kulas and Stachowski 2009;Murray et al 2015;Onwuegbuzie and Weems 2004). Also, the probability of chosing a central value in the scale is much lower than chosing any other value even for people whose trait levels are central (González-Romá and Espejo 2003;Hernández et al 2006).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Additionally, many studies question central values in response scales given that the verbal statement that acompanies the central number does not mean the individual considers they are in the middle of the continuum of the corresponding latent construct (Dalal et al 2014;González-Romá and Espejo 2003;Hernández et al Kulas et al 2008;Kulas and Stachowski 2009;Murray et al 2015;Onwuegbuzie and Weems 2004). Also, the probability of chosing a central value in the scale is much lower than chosing any other value even for people whose trait levels are central (González-Romá and Espejo 2003;Hernández et al 2006).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In each of these studies the set of items were selected to be very similar, or identical, and have also possessed a neutral or mildly positive valence. As these items may not be intrinsically interesting it is possible that when people are forced to choose between them they select the middle item as their default option because of indifference toward the items, as can occur on some rating scales (see Kulas, Stachowski, & Haynes, 2008). Similarly, as proposed by Christenfeld (1995), limited interest in the items may also cause participants to expend minimal mental effort on the selection, with the middle item selected because it appears to be the easiest option and requires the least thought.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because the middle response option does not always represent a neutral response, this can undermine the analysis of the latent trait and present false correlations, as well as provide a sort of "dumping ground" for when participants are uncertain of the item or their response (Kulas, Stachowski, & Haynes, 2008;Kulas & Stachowski, 2009). We gave one sample of participants the original 5-option survey and another sample a 6- Factor Structure of FFMQ Models fit to the standard 39-item dataset (D1-D3 combined, n = 725), showed that the correlated five-factor model with both positive and negative method effects provided the best fit to the data (see Table 1), consistent with prior work using a smaller subset of this sample (Van Dam et al 2012).…”
Section: Response Formattingmentioning
confidence: 99%