2011
DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0035
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Morphology and behaviour: functional links in development and evolution

Abstract: Development and evolution of animal behaviour and morphology are frequently addressed independently, as reflected in the dichotomy of disciplines dedicated to their study distinguishing object of study (morphology versus behaviour) and perspective (ultimate versus proximate). Although traits are known to develop and evolve semi-independently, they are matched together in development and evolution to produce a unique functional phenotype. Here I highlight similarities shared by both traits, such as the decisive… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
57
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 72 publications
(57 citation statements)
references
References 113 publications
0
57
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Identifying evolutionary and ecological patterns of morphological and behavioral variation is critical to understanding the nature of diversification [Bertossa, 2011;Parsons et al, 2012;Weber et al, 2013]. Morphological, behavioral, and neuroanatomical differentiation are predicted to be coupled, with brain organization enabling adaptive responses to ecological challenges [Dukas, 1998;Farris and Roberts, 2005;Striedter, 2005;Tsuboi et al, 2014].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Identifying evolutionary and ecological patterns of morphological and behavioral variation is critical to understanding the nature of diversification [Bertossa, 2011;Parsons et al, 2012;Weber et al, 2013]. Morphological, behavioral, and neuroanatomical differentiation are predicted to be coupled, with brain organization enabling adaptive responses to ecological challenges [Dukas, 1998;Farris and Roberts, 2005;Striedter, 2005;Tsuboi et al, 2014].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Behaviour, which is often evolutionarily conserved, is also a proper subject of homology relations and can be used in phylogenetic reconstruction (Rendall and Di Fiore 2007;Bertossa 2011;Hall 2013). Although behavior is developmentally labile, dependent on environmental input, and realized via disparate ontogenetic pathways, the same holds true of morphology, and thus these properties do not preclude its 'homologization'.…”
Section: Homology In Cultural Evolutionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Similarly, control could be handed over from genetic programs to epigenetic or cultural determinants, or vice versa. Unfortunately for this neat account, the picture of genes as the locus of special causal agency in development is mistaken (Oyama 2000;Griffiths 2001;Robert 2004;Pigliucci 2010;Bertossa 2011). Genes are one amongst many causal factors that make a contingent difference to organismic development (Oyama 2000).…”
Section: Homology Across Inheritance Systems In Principlementioning
confidence: 99%
“…To be sure, precise predictions about the future of evo-devo are impossible (Hall, 2012), but the possibility, and in consequence the responsibility, is to an extent in our hands to steer the boat toward intellectually rewarding paths. We are already witnessing our discipline expanding its scope by exploring its boundaries toward other disciplines, ecology in particular (eco-evodevo, e.g., Hall, 2003;Gilbert and Epel, 2008; also evo-devo vs. niche construction, e.g., Laland et al, 2008), but also ethology (e.g., Bertossa, 2011) and the sciences of language (e.g., Hoang et al, 2011;Hall, 2013). Future advances in all these directions are welcome, but I expect that much more progress will come from the impact that evo-devo will eventually have on the foundations and the research agendas of its parent disciplines.…”
Section: Questions To Be Addressedmentioning
confidence: 99%