2018
DOI: 10.1121/1.5042759
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Multisensory stimuli improve relative localisation judgments compared to unisensory auditory or visual stimuli

Abstract: Observers performed a relative localisation task in which they reported whether the second of two sequentially presented signals occurred to the left or right of the first. Stimuli were detectability-matched auditory, visual, or auditory-visual signals and the goal was to compare changes in performance with eccentricity across modalities. Visual performance was superior to auditory at the midline, but inferior in the periphery, while auditory-visual performance exceeded both at all locations. No such advantage… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

1
6
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
1
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We assume that the present results can be explained in terms of the lack of retinovisuomotor calibration of the auditory space with blindness and by the specificities of the multisensory mechanisms of spatial calibration that replace visual calibration in the blind (Lewald, 2002a(Lewald, , 2002b(Lewald, , 2013Zwiers et al, 2001aZwiers et al, , 2001bZwiers et al, , 2003. In this context, it is important to note that in normalsighted persons, visual localization acuity is maximal in the foveal region and decreases substantially with increasing eccentricity (Charbonneau et al, 2013;Freeman, Wood, & Bizley, 2018;Mateeff & Gourevich, 1983;Perrott, Costantino, & Cisneros, 1993). As mentioned earlier, auditory localization acuity also increases with increasing eccentricity (Blauert, 1997;Carlile et al, 2016;Charbonneau et al, 2013;Freeman et al, 2018;Makous & Middlebrooks, 1990;Mills, 1958;R€ oder et al, 1999;Wood & Bizley, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 63%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We assume that the present results can be explained in terms of the lack of retinovisuomotor calibration of the auditory space with blindness and by the specificities of the multisensory mechanisms of spatial calibration that replace visual calibration in the blind (Lewald, 2002a(Lewald, , 2002b(Lewald, , 2013Zwiers et al, 2001aZwiers et al, , 2001bZwiers et al, , 2003. In this context, it is important to note that in normalsighted persons, visual localization acuity is maximal in the foveal region and decreases substantially with increasing eccentricity (Charbonneau et al, 2013;Freeman, Wood, & Bizley, 2018;Mateeff & Gourevich, 1983;Perrott, Costantino, & Cisneros, 1993). As mentioned earlier, auditory localization acuity also increases with increasing eccentricity (Blauert, 1997;Carlile et al, 2016;Charbonneau et al, 2013;Freeman et al, 2018;Makous & Middlebrooks, 1990;Mills, 1958;R€ oder et al, 1999;Wood & Bizley, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 63%
“…In this context, it is important to note that in normalsighted persons, visual localization acuity is maximal in the foveal region and decreases substantially with increasing eccentricity (Charbonneau et al, 2013;Freeman, Wood, & Bizley, 2018;Mateeff & Gourevich, 1983;Perrott, Costantino, & Cisneros, 1993). As mentioned earlier, auditory localization acuity also increases with increasing eccentricity (Blauert, 1997;Carlile et al, 2016;Charbonneau et al, 2013;Freeman et al, 2018;Makous & Middlebrooks, 1990;Mills, 1958;R€ oder et al, 1999;Wood & Bizley, 2015). Furthermore, the cross-modal bias (i.e., the ability of a visual signal to influence the localization of an auditory target) and the ventriloquism effect (i.e., the visual capture of sounds with presentation of spatially incongruent bimodal targets) have been shown to be maximal at central locations and to decrease with eccentricity (Charbonneau et al, 2013;Hairston et al, 2003;Lewald & Guski, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In virtual audio-visual scenes the audio is not presented in isolation, but in combination with visual information, which is known to strongly influence sound localization (e.g., [15][16][17]). When audio and visual stimuli are presented in close temporal and spatial proximity, they are integrated into one common percept, increasing the accuracy and precision of localization (e.g., [18][19][20]). As a result of this process, when the audio and visual stimuli are not exactly at the same position, but still integrated, the perceived location of the auditory stimuli is shifted strongly towards that of the visual stimulus (the so called 'ventriloquist effect', e.g., [18,[21][22][23]).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In virtual audio-visual scenes the audio is not presented in isolation, but in combination with visual information, which is known to strongly influence sound localization (e.g., Dufour et al, 2002;Tabry et al, 2013;Gori et al, 2014). When audio and visual stimuli are presented in close temporal and spatial proximity, they are integrated into one common percept, increasing the accuracy and precision of localization (e.g., Alais and Burr, 2004;Odegaard et al, 2015;Freeman et al, 2018). As a result of this process, when the audio and visual stimuli are not exactly at the same position, but still integrated, the perceived location of the auditory stimuli is shifted strongly towards that of the visual stimulus (the so called "ventriloquist effect,", e.g., Alais and Burr, 2004;Jackson, 1953;Lewald and Guski, 2003;Thurlow and Jack, 1973).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%