2017
DOI: 10.1080/17511321.2017.1294197
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Muscular Imaginings—A Phenomenological and Enactive Model for Imagination

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Rather, the learner is seen as someone depicted (by the nonenactivists) as “jerking” between a string of “deliberate practices” (Robertson & Hutto, 2022). Anders Ericsson, the originator of the idea of “deliberate practice,” says that Csikszentmihalyi (1990) “flow” (or “mushin,” in Japanese) is “incompatible with deliberate practice” (Ericsson & Ward, 2007, p. 349); it is, however, central to radical enactivist accounts (Ilundáin-Agurruza, 2017; Krein & Ilundáin-Agurruza, 2017). Radical enactivists suggest that context-sensitive habits are the building blocks of skilled performance and that they become part of the flow due to imagination.…”
Section: Applicationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Rather, the learner is seen as someone depicted (by the nonenactivists) as “jerking” between a string of “deliberate practices” (Robertson & Hutto, 2022). Anders Ericsson, the originator of the idea of “deliberate practice,” says that Csikszentmihalyi (1990) “flow” (or “mushin,” in Japanese) is “incompatible with deliberate practice” (Ericsson & Ward, 2007, p. 349); it is, however, central to radical enactivist accounts (Ilundáin-Agurruza, 2017; Krein & Ilundáin-Agurruza, 2017). Radical enactivists suggest that context-sensitive habits are the building blocks of skilled performance and that they become part of the flow due to imagination.…”
Section: Applicationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Before climbing, he spends 2 days "sitting and thinking, hour after hour. Visualising every single move, everything that could possibly happen" (Honnold and Roberts, 2016;Ilundáin-Agurruza, 2017). Honnold's imagining process is a case of CI, since it is not only pictorial, but it involves kinetic, tactile, kinesthetic, nociceptive, and other sensory modalities that are deeply embodied in nature (see Ilundáin-Agurruza, 2017).…”
Section: Switching Paradigm: Embodying Mind Wanderingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Visualising every single move, everything that could possibly happen" (Honnold and Roberts, 2016;Ilundáin-Agurruza, 2017). Honnold's imagining process is a case of CI, since it is not only pictorial, but it involves kinetic, tactile, kinesthetic, nociceptive, and other sensory modalities that are deeply embodied in nature (see Ilundáin-Agurruza, 2017). Furthermore, eidetic imagining is, according to Ilundáin-Agarruza, dependent on corporeal imagining (see also Dewey, 1980).…”
Section: Switching Paradigm: Embodying Mind Wanderingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Enactivism also works with findings from neuroscience, where mental imagery is characterized not as a representational process, but as a motoric, bodily process involving neuronal activation and engaging bodily movements (Jeannerod, 1997;Driskell et al, 1994;Guillot et al, 2012;see Gallagher & Rucińska, 2021). Available proposals for embodied and enactive imagination (Hutto, 2015;Hutto & Myin, 2013Ilundáin-Agurruza, 2017;Medina, 2013) treat imagination as a form of action that is strongly integrated with perceiving, involves implicit embodied sensorimotor processes, and is densely textured in a cross-modal (kinetic, tactile, kinesthetic and olfactory) way. Thus, the imaginative processes, including mental imagery, are seen by enactivists as grounded in embodied motor activations, which can further explain re-enactments in pretend play.…”
Section: The Mind Of the Pretendermentioning
confidence: 99%