1995
DOI: 10.3758/bf03210958
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

7
393
3
4

Year Published

1996
1996
2008
2008

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 456 publications
(407 citation statements)
references
References 104 publications
7
393
3
4
Order By: Relevance
“…The only difference between our procedure and hers was that participants read the words on the probe trial rather than performing a lexical decision. We asked participants to read the words because the majority of NP tasks employ word-reading or color-naming rather than lexical decision (e.g., May et al, 1995, Enright et al, 1993, 1995MacDonald et al, 1999;McNally et al, 2001). NP scores were calculated by subtracting the latency to name the target word when primed by X's (i.e., control trials) from the response latency to name the same target word when primed by itself (i.e., ignored repetition trials).…”
Section: Design and Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The only difference between our procedure and hers was that participants read the words on the probe trial rather than performing a lexical decision. We asked participants to read the words because the majority of NP tasks employ word-reading or color-naming rather than lexical decision (e.g., May et al, 1995, Enright et al, 1993, 1995MacDonald et al, 1999;McNally et al, 2001). NP scores were calculated by subtracting the latency to name the target word when primed by X's (i.e., control trials) from the response latency to name the same target word when primed by itself (i.e., ignored repetition trials).…”
Section: Design and Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is a large body of research establishing the NP effect (Dalrymple-Alford & Budayr, 1966;Neill, 1977;Tipper, 1985; for reviews, see Fox, 1995;May, Kane, & Hasher, 1995;Kane, May, Hasher, Rahhal, & Stoltzfus, 1997;Tipper, 2001). However, disagreements over interpretation of the finding persist (e.g., Frings & Wentura, 2006;MacLeod, Chiappe, & Fox, 2002;Tipper, 2001).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Response times (RTs) on these "ignored repetition" (IR) trials are usually slower than those on control (C) trials. Since the seminal studies of Neill (1977) and Tipper (1985), a large amount ofresearch has demonstrated that negative priming occurs across a range of stimuli (words, letters, pictures, numbers, nonsense shapes), as well as across a wide variety of task demands (categorization, matching, counting, and target localization; for comprehensive reviews, see Fox, 1995;May, Kane, & Hasher, 1995;Neill & Valdes, 1996;Neill, Valdes, & Terry, 1995;Tipper & Milliken, 1996). Although the methodological variations across many studies have been valuable in demonstrating the generality of negative priming, this diversity has also created difficulties in drawing comparisons across studies (see, e.g., Fox, 1995;May et aI., 1995;Tipper & Milliken, 1996).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Few, if any, NP studies control for the confound with response selection, despite the fact that it is present in the most common procedures, including those used with special populations (Hasher, Stoltzfus, Zacks, & Rypma, 1991;Kane, Hasher, Stoltzfus, Zacks, & Connelly, 1994;Metzler & Parkin, 2000;Tipper, 1991). Here, we discuss two specific NP paradigms to make this point clearer: location-based NP and Stroop NP (see Fox, 1995, andTipper, 2001, for more detailed reviews and descriptions of various procedures).…”
Section: On the Locus Of Negative Priming: Is There A Role For Responmentioning
confidence: 99%