Computational accounts of reading aloud largely ignore context when stipulating how processing unfolds. One exception to this state of affairs proposes adjusting the breadth of lexical knowledge in such models in response to differing contexts. Three experiments and corresponding simulations, using Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, and Ziegler's (2001) dual-route cascaded model, are reported. This work investigates a determinant of when a pseudohomophone such as brane is affected by the frequency of the word from which it is derived (e.g., the base word frequency of brain) by examining performance under conditions where it is read aloud faster than a nonword control such as frane. Reynolds and Besner's (2005a) lexical breadth account makes the novel prediction that when a pseudohomophone advantage is seen, there will also be a base word frequency effect, provided exception words are also present. This prediction was confirmed. Five other accounts of this pattern of results are considered and found wanting. It is concluded that the lexical breadth account provides the most parsimonious account to date of these and related findings.Keywords Context effects . Lexical processing . Reading aloud . Word recognition . Automaticity . Automatic processing . Pseudohomophones How do skilled readers use lexical knowledge when reading words and orthographically novel letter strings? Recent evidence suggests that a complete answer to this question requires a consideration of context. For instance, list context influences (1) how letter-level processing and lexical-level processing communicate with one another (e.g., Besner & O'Malley, 2009;Besner, O'Malley, & Robidoux, 2010;O'Malley & Besner, 2008) and (2) how semantics affects visual word recognition (e.g., Brown, Stolz, & Besner, 2006;Robidoux, Stolz, & Besner, 2010;Ferguson, Robidoux, & Besner, 2009;Stolz & Neely, 1995). Other work examining print-to-sound translation suggests that context also affects (3) how the breadth of lexical knowledge contributes to the generation of a phonological code. According to Reynolds and Besner (2005a), the breadth of the lexical contribution can vary from narrow, where relatively few entries in the mental lexicon are activated, to broad, where many such entries are activated. The present study derives, tests, and confirms a novel prediction of this account for reading aloud.
Reading aloudThe currently most successful theories of how pronounceable letter strings are read aloud postulate two pathways for translating print into sound (Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, & Ziegler, 2001;Perry, Ziegler, & Zorzi, 2007 Mem Cogn (2011) 39:1332-1347 For instance, in Coltheart and colleagues' dual-route cascaded (DRC) model, the nonlexical pathway (pathway B in Fig. 1) assembles a phonological code, using grapheme-to-phoneme rules (e.g., 'th' → /T/). This pathway generates a correct pronunciation for regular words (e.g., hint) and nonwords (e.g., zint) but regularizes exception words (e.g., pint is read so as to rhyme with hint). The lexi...