2003
DOI: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00162-9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Neural mechanisms of top-down control during spatial and feature attention

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

57
285
3
3

Year Published

2004
2004
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 355 publications
(348 citation statements)
references
References 77 publications
57
285
3
3
Order By: Relevance
“…The more inferior medial activation of the precuneus corresponds closely to areas reported previously to be active during spatial attention and spatial working memory tasks [Corbetta et al, 1993;Giesbrecht et al, 2003;Owen et al, 1996] as well as during temporal orienting and combined spatial and temporal orienting [Coull and Nobre, 1998]. We propose that, together with the SPL, it serves to maintain a spatiotemporal bias against to-be-ignored locations, in favor of to-be-searched locations.…”
Section: Discussion Previewsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…The more inferior medial activation of the precuneus corresponds closely to areas reported previously to be active during spatial attention and spatial working memory tasks [Corbetta et al, 1993;Giesbrecht et al, 2003;Owen et al, 1996] as well as during temporal orienting and combined spatial and temporal orienting [Coull and Nobre, 1998]. We propose that, together with the SPL, it serves to maintain a spatiotemporal bias against to-be-ignored locations, in favor of to-be-searched locations.…”
Section: Discussion Previewsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…Many previous studies have implicated these regions (particularly the DLPFC) in top-down control of processing, including the domains of selective attention (Mathalon et al, 2004;Milham et al, 2003), task-switching (Aron et al, 2004;Sussman et al, 2003), spatial attention (Giesbrecht et al, 2003;, sustained attention (Lawrence et al, 2003;Ortuno et al, 2002), emotional (Boshuisen et al, 2002;Milham et al, 2003), and autonomic control (Critchley et al, 2002(Critchley et al, , 2003. The finding that activation in this region (along with the right DLPFC) was significantly greater for No-go trials when compared to the Go trials is also consistent with the studies suggesting it monitors for response conflict Carter et al, 2001), whereby conflict signals the need for the allocation of additional control by the DLPFC (MacDonald et al, 2000).…”
Section: Pfc and Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Interindividual differences in MFG activation may thus reflect different performance strategies, such that participants with lower activity had progressed to a more automatic performance mode. Parahippocampal gyrus activity increases with attention to shapes (Corbetta et al, 1991a;b); thus, lower activity with faster responding during anglediscriminations may, again, reflect performance benefits of an increasing automaticity. But left parahippocampal gyrus also activates with reading color words (Pulvermueller and Hauk, 2006), and right parahippocampal gyrus with color imagery (Howard et al, 1998).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Not surprisingly, brain regions displaying strongest activation during DIV included those taxed by top-down control of spatial and non-spatial (e.g. based on color) attentional selection, such as intraparietal sulcus and MFG (Hopfinger et al, 2000;Corbetta et al, 2000;Giesbrecht et al, 2003). However, control demands can differ greatly between different divided attention conditions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%