2009
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0812142106
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers

Abstract: Although infants and animals respond to the approximate number of elements in visual, auditory, and tactile arrays, only human children and adults have been shown to possess abstract numerical representations that apply to entities of all kinds (e.g., 7 samurai, seas, or sins). Do abstract numerical concepts depend on language or culture, or do they form a part of humans' innate, core knowledge? Here we show that newborn infants spontaneously associate stationary, visual-spatial arrays of 4 -18 objects with au… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

23
550
4
19

Year Published

2012
2012
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 685 publications
(596 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
23
550
4
19
Order By: Relevance
“…As development proceeds, however, the system becomes more finely tuned to smaller differences in quantities. Consistent with this, infants as young as 48 h are able to differentiate ratios of 3:1 but not 2:1 (20), and, as they age, children show increasingly precise abilities: at 6 mo, they can distinguish ratios of 2:1, and by 9 mo, ratios of 3:2 (21,22). They eventually reach competencies for ratios 4:3 by 3 y, 6:5 by 6 y, and 8:7 and more difficult ratios by adulthood (23)(24)(25)(26).…”
supporting
confidence: 70%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…As development proceeds, however, the system becomes more finely tuned to smaller differences in quantities. Consistent with this, infants as young as 48 h are able to differentiate ratios of 3:1 but not 2:1 (20), and, as they age, children show increasingly precise abilities: at 6 mo, they can distinguish ratios of 2:1, and by 9 mo, ratios of 3:2 (21,22). They eventually reach competencies for ratios 4:3 by 3 y, 6:5 by 6 y, and 8:7 and more difficult ratios by adulthood (23)(24)(25)(26).…”
supporting
confidence: 70%
“…Specifically, we expect the subcortical mechanism to respond selectively to nonsymbolic quantities (here, dot arrays) given that symbolic manipulation of quantities is uniquely human and likely requires cortical contributions. Moreover, we predict that subcortical contributions to number processing will be ratio dependent as young children evince numerical abilities that are themselves ratio dependent (20)(21)(22).…”
mentioning
confidence: 92%
“…The ANS is believed to be shared by many species and would allow the discrimination, the comparison, the addition, and the subtraction of numerosities presented in and across different formats and modalities (i.e. comparing the number of objects seen or touched, the number of tones or voices, the number of perceived events; Féron, Gentaz, & Streri, 2006;Izard, Sann, Spelke, & Streri, 2009;Kobayashi, Hiraki, & Hasegawa, 2005). The resulting approximate number representation is therefore considered to be independent of the modality (Barth, Kanwisher, & Spelke, 2003;Barth et al, 2006;Meck & Church, 1983).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Also known as the approximate number system (ANS), this nonsymbolic sense of numerical magnitude is shared with nonhuman animals (6) and is widespread across cultures (7). Unlike the acquisition of symbolic number (e.g., Arabic digits) and formal math concepts, which are learned via explicit instruction and allow for exact quantification, the ANS may be innate (8) and is characteristically "noisy," with variance increasing linearly as a function of the absolute numerical value (9). This imprecision can be modeled as overlapping Gaussian distributions along an internal continuum (10) and is captured by Weber's law, which holds that subjective differences in intensity are proportional to the objective ratios between values.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%