2008
DOI: 10.1080/14622200802097514
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Nicotine decreases attentional bias to negative-affect-related Stroop words among smokers

Abstract: The present study examined the hypothesis that nicotine is associated with reduced attentional bias to affective and smoking-related stimuli in a modified Stroop task. A total of 56 habitual smokers were each tested on 4 days with 14 mg nicotine patches and placebo patches, counterbalanced, as a within-subjects factor in a double-blind design. A modified Stroop using negative-affect words, smoking words, color words, and neutral words was presented via computer in blocked format. As predicted, nicotine, relati… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

3
16
2

Year Published

2010
2010
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
3
16
2
Order By: Relevance
“…that current smokers would demonstrate an attentional bias through slower reactions times in the presence of drug-related cues. While smokers have been shown to demonstrate state-dependent (abstinence vs. satiety) alterations in arousal (Parrott and Kaye, 1999), motivation (Powell et al, 2002) and sustained attention (Rusted et al, 2000), which might bestow an acute beneficial effect of nicotine on task performance, it may be that this lack of behavioural effect represents the small sample size used in Experiment 1, compared to previous studies (Domier et al, 2007;Munafo et al, 2003;Rzetelny et al, 2008). Experiment 2 investigated neural activity during motor response inhibition and error monitoring in the same sample using a go/no-go task.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 86%
“…that current smokers would demonstrate an attentional bias through slower reactions times in the presence of drug-related cues. While smokers have been shown to demonstrate state-dependent (abstinence vs. satiety) alterations in arousal (Parrott and Kaye, 1999), motivation (Powell et al, 2002) and sustained attention (Rusted et al, 2000), which might bestow an acute beneficial effect of nicotine on task performance, it may be that this lack of behavioural effect represents the small sample size used in Experiment 1, compared to previous studies (Domier et al, 2007;Munafo et al, 2003;Rzetelny et al, 2008). Experiment 2 investigated neural activity during motor response inhibition and error monitoring in the same sample using a go/no-go task.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 86%
“…Consistent with this model, nicotine withdrawal potentiates the startle response (Cinciripini et al , 2006) and amplifies electrocortical responses (Gilbert et al , 2004) to negative emotional stimuli, and reduces the hedonic rating of positive stimuli (Dawkins et al , 2007). Moreover, data from a small number of studies suggest nicotine withdrawal results in greater interference from emotional stimuli during task-relevant processes (Drobes et al , 2006; Gilbert et al , 2008; Gilbert et al , 2007; Powell et al , 2002; Rzetelny et al , 2008). Despite these findings, the neural mechanisms underlying the interaction between smoking abstinence and emotion–cognition remain poorly characterized.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous studies indicate that smokers exhibit an attentional bias to smoking and negative affect stimuli (Drobes et al, 2006), smoking abstinence increases attentional bias towards negative affect stimuli (Rzetelny et al, 2008), and attentional bias to smoking cues predicts abstinence during a cessation attempt (Waters, Shiffman et al, 2003). However, speeded initial orienting to stimuli (IO) and the inability to disengage from stimuli (ITD) have been identified as distinct components of attentional bias (Field, Munafo, & Franken, 2009), and these previous studies did not distinguish between the IO and ITD.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%