2000
DOI: 10.1080/10413200008404215
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Normative values for the profile of mood states for use with athletic samples

Abstract: The Profile of Mood States (POMS) has been used extensively for the assessment of mood in the sport and exercise environments. The purpose of the study was to develop tables of normative values based on athletic samples. Participants (N = 2,086), comprising athletes at the international (n = 622), club (n = 628), and recreational (n = 836) levels, completed the POMS in one of three situations: precompetition/exercise, post-competition/exercise, and away from the athletic environment. Differences between the at… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

6
83
0
2

Year Published

2000
2000
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 98 publications
(91 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
6
83
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Terry (1995a) proposed that individual differences in skill and conditioning make it "entirely unreasonable" (p.310) to expect mood to predict athletic achievement and Renger (1993) went as far as calling for researchers to "abandon the POMS" (p.83) in research on successful and unsuccessful athletes. However, given that some findings are supportive of the differentiation of athletic achievement from mood scores (e.g., Morgan, Brown, Raglin, O'Connor, & Ellickson, 1987;Terry & Hall, 1996;Trafton, Meyers, & Skelly, 1998), it appears that this research question has yet to be answered fully.The suggestion that POMS scores are predictive of performance among athletes of homogeneous ability is perhaps the most intuitively reliable association, yet a definitive answer to this research question has also proved elusive. It has been proposed by Terry (1995a) that the prediction of performance from mood is maximized when situational variables which potentially moderate the mood-performance link are considered.…”
mentioning
confidence: 87%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Terry (1995a) proposed that individual differences in skill and conditioning make it "entirely unreasonable" (p.310) to expect mood to predict athletic achievement and Renger (1993) went as far as calling for researchers to "abandon the POMS" (p.83) in research on successful and unsuccessful athletes. However, given that some findings are supportive of the differentiation of athletic achievement from mood scores (e.g., Morgan, Brown, Raglin, O'Connor, & Ellickson, 1987;Terry & Hall, 1996;Trafton, Meyers, & Skelly, 1998), it appears that this research question has yet to be answered fully.The suggestion that POMS scores are predictive of performance among athletes of homogeneous ability is perhaps the most intuitively reliable association, yet a definitive answer to this research question has also proved elusive. It has been proposed by Terry (1995a) that the prediction of performance from mood is maximized when situational variables which potentially moderate the mood-performance link are considered.…”
mentioning
confidence: 87%
“…In respect of the first research question, qualitative reviews of the extant literature by LeUnes, Haywood, and Daiss (1988), Renger (1993), and Vanden Auweele, De Cuyper, Van Mele, and Rzewnicki (1993) demonstrate clearly that athletes typically report iceberg profiles, which by definition vary from population norms derived largely from nonathletes. Further, recently published normative data based on the mood responses of 2,086 POMS Meta-analysis 5 participants in sport and exercise (see Terry & Hall, 1996) confirmed that an iceberg profile is "normal" for athletes, thereby supporting Morgan's mental health model.In respect of the second research question, reliable conclusions have been far more elusive. Terry (1995a) proposed that individual differences in skill and conditioning make it "entirely unreasonable" (p.310) to expect mood to predict athletic achievement and Renger (1993) went as far as calling for researchers to "abandon the POMS" (p.83) in research on successful and unsuccessful athletes.…”
mentioning
confidence: 87%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The emotional responses that underlie mood change are initiated by our interaction with other individuals and environmental forces that we encounter as we move from situation to situation. Few studies have investigated the effects of situational factors on mood, particularly in athletic environments, although in a cross-sectional study of the mood responses of more than 2000 athletes assessed either before competition, after competition or away from the competition environment, Terry and Lane (2000) found that athletes reported higher tension, depression, anger and confusion prior to competition than at the post-competition stage. Away from the competition environment, such feelings were at levels midway between the pre-and postcompetition situations.…”
Section: Measurement Issuesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Further, normative mood data for athletes published by Terry and Lane (2000) showed that, when plotted against the psychiatric outpatient or student norms (see McNair et al, 1971) that were used as the point of reference by almost all previous studies, an iceberg profile is typical of athletes regardless of their level of competition. Collectively, the more recent evidence does not mean that previous emphasis on the desirability of the iceberg profile was misplaced but, given the normality of such a profile among athletes, perhaps its importance was overstated.…”
Section: Measurement Issuesmentioning
confidence: 99%