2007
DOI: 10.3758/bf03193761
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Object and spatial representations in the corner enhancement effect

Abstract: Cole, Gellatly, and Blurton (2001) have shown that targets presented adjacent to geometric corners are detected more efficiently than targets presented adjacent to straight edges. In six experiments, we examined how this corner enhancement effect is modulated by corner-of-object representations (i.e., corners that define an object's shape) and local base-level corners that occur as a result of, for instance, overlapping the straight edges of two objects. The results show that the corner phenomenon is greater f… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This happens to be on the same side to which the arrows point and could itself induce an attention shift. This notion is supported by work showing that the internal structure of an object is important in influencing the distribution of attention within the object (Reppa & Leek, 2003;Vecera, Behrmann, & McGoldrick, 2000) as well as beyond its borders (Cole, Gellatly, & Blurton, 2001;Cole, Skarratt, & Gellatly, 2007). Thus, although the results of Experiments 3A, 3B, and 4 show that a shift in spatial attention can induce a negative compatibility-like effect, the primes presented in Experiment 2 may not have been acting as a spatial cue in the purest sense of the word.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 80%
“…This happens to be on the same side to which the arrows point and could itself induce an attention shift. This notion is supported by work showing that the internal structure of an object is important in influencing the distribution of attention within the object (Reppa & Leek, 2003;Vecera, Behrmann, & McGoldrick, 2000) as well as beyond its borders (Cole, Gellatly, & Blurton, 2001;Cole, Skarratt, & Gellatly, 2007). Thus, although the results of Experiments 3A, 3B, and 4 show that a shift in spatial attention can induce a negative compatibility-like effect, the primes presented in Experiment 2 may not have been acting as a spatial cue in the purest sense of the word.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 80%
“…Responses to angular shapes were fast in both conditions (towards and away), and it is possible that these shapes attracted attention or were more arousing than the curved shapes. This result is in line with the corner enhancement effect (Cole et al ., ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In general, a role for curvature extrema is well acknowledged in theories of shape representation (Cohen & Singh, ; De Winter & Wagemans, , ; Feldman & Singh, ; Hoffman & Richards, ; Hoffman & Singh, ; Leyton, ). There is also evidence that attention is allocated preferentially to regions near corners (Bertamini, Helmy, & Bates, ; Cole, Skarratt, & Gellatly, ) perhaps because these locations are more informative.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In general, a role for curvature extrema is acknowledged in theories of shape representation (Cohen & Singh, 2007; De Winter & Wagemans, 2006, 2008; Feldman & Singh, 2005; Hoffman & Richards, 1984; Hoffman & Singh, 1997; Leyton, 1989). There is also evidence that attention is allocated preferentially to regions near corners (Bertamini, Helmy, & Bates, 2013; Cole, Skarratt, & Gellatly, 2007), perhaps because these are more informative locations. Many studies have also highlighted how positive and negative extrema (convexities and concavities) carry information about solid shape (Bertamini & Wagemans, 2013; Hoffman & Richards, 1984; Koenderink, 1984).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%