1974
DOI: 10.1038/250732a0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Olfactory imprinting resulting from brief exposure in Acomys cahirinus

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
11
0

Year Published

1975
1975
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 70 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
0
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Unfortunately, in pups incapable of locomotion, motor activity could signify approach or withdrawal just as kicking can indicate interest or impatience (Thelen, 1981). Although behavioral activation does not reveal the pup’s affective response to the odor, numerous past studies with various species have reported that mere exposure increases preference for that odor (Balogh & Porter, 1986; Hepper, 1988; Porter & Etscorn, 1974; Zajonc, 1968). Unfortunately, our attempts to obtain satisfactorily reliable measures of preference in the first hours after birth have so far been unsuccessful.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Unfortunately, in pups incapable of locomotion, motor activity could signify approach or withdrawal just as kicking can indicate interest or impatience (Thelen, 1981). Although behavioral activation does not reveal the pup’s affective response to the odor, numerous past studies with various species have reported that mere exposure increases preference for that odor (Balogh & Porter, 1986; Hepper, 1988; Porter & Etscorn, 1974; Zajonc, 1968). Unfortunately, our attempts to obtain satisfactorily reliable measures of preference in the first hours after birth have so far been unsuccessful.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…In addition to traditional imprinting usually restricted to fowl, researchers have studied mammalian imprinting in several different species (rats, Hudson, 1993; deer, Muller-Schwarze & Muller-Schwarze, 1971; spiny mice, Porter & Etscorn, 1974; guinea pigs, Sluckin, 1968). Instead of a moving object, mammals typically imprint onto an olfactory stimulus, usually maternally derived odors.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We discover social experience-dependent changes in dopamine (DA) and gamma aminobutyric acid (GABA) expression in granule and periglomerular interneurons of the AOB affecting kinship recognition and preference. These findings may shed light on the olfactory imprinting of fish (Gerlach et al, 2008; Harden et al, 2006; Hino et al, 2009; Johnstone et al, 2011) and neonatal attachment in mammals (Janus, 1993; Kendrick et al, 1997a; Porter and Etscorn, 1974), potentially including humans.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Newborn young 2-12 hr old show a rapid olfactory imprinting, since a brief, 1-hr exposure to a chemical cue induced a preference for that artificial odor 24 hr later (Porter and Etscorn, 1974). One-day-old pups show a preference for bedding soiled by their mother over bedding soiled by a nonpregnant female, but they show no preference for bedding soiled by their mother over that of another lactating female (Porter and Doane, 1976;Porter and Ruttle, 1975), This suggests the presence of a maternal pheromone as in rats and house mice (see above).…”
Section: Rodentsmentioning
confidence: 99%