2013
DOI: 10.7771/1932-6246.1152
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On Evaluating Human Problem Solving of Computationally Hard Problems

Abstract: This article is concerned with how computer science, and more exactly computational complexity theory, can inform cognitive science. In particular, we suggest factors to be taken into account when investigating how people deal with computational hardness. This discussion will address the two upper levels of Marr's Level Theory: the computational level and the algorithmic level. Our reasons for believing that humans indeed deal with hard cognitive functions are threefold: (1) Several computationally hard functi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 58 publications
(78 reference statements)
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…(Chi, Glaser, & Rees, 1982). This is reflected in the relevant literature such as Newell and Simon (1972), and more recently in many contributions in the Journal of Problem Solving-compare discussions in Carruthers andStege (2013) andFischer, Greiff, andFunke (2012), and in introductory reviews such as Anderson (2004), which focus on the complex high-level operations that need to be mentally organised, based on the range of possible actions and problem states. It is in this area that think-aloud protocols as data sources have been most successful (Ericsson & Simon, 1984).…”
Section: Abstract: Problem Solving Instructions Text Interpretationmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…(Chi, Glaser, & Rees, 1982). This is reflected in the relevant literature such as Newell and Simon (1972), and more recently in many contributions in the Journal of Problem Solving-compare discussions in Carruthers andStege (2013) andFischer, Greiff, andFunke (2012), and in introductory reviews such as Anderson (2004), which focus on the complex high-level operations that need to be mentally organised, based on the range of possible actions and problem states. It is in this area that think-aloud protocols as data sources have been most successful (Ericsson & Simon, 1984).…”
Section: Abstract: Problem Solving Instructions Text Interpretationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Accordingly, much of the problem solving literature addresses how people identify problems and operators to solve them, and how these operators are ordered into a sequence of actions so as to reach a suitable solution, mediated by expertise (Chi, Glaser, & Rees, 1982). This is reflected in the relevant literature such as Newell and Simon (1972), and more recently in many contributions in the Journal of Problem Solving-compare discussions in Carruthers and Stege (2013) and Fischer, Greiff, and Funke (2012), and in introductory reviews such as Anderson (2004), which focus on the complex high-level operations that need to be mentally organised, based on the range of possible actions and problem states.…”
Section: Mental Processes In Problem Solving Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research in different fields highlighted the role of games in favouring the connection between abstraction and formalization. Games designed with a purpose (GWAPs) can be powerful tools to boost research and science education [29][30][31][32][37][38][39][40][41][42][43], in particular favouring the use of intuition to quickly solve very complex computational problems [44], and reinforcing motivations and voluntary participation, ensuring…”
Section: Data Availability Statementmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The design of pertinent experiments is an enormous challenge and even with ideal circumstances for testing we cannot be entirely sure that we have accurately identified the algorithmic procedures. While that does not mean that we cannot gather reliable data at all, it does bring in the kind of inexactness that is a bad fit with the exact notions involved in the study of computational complexity (see, e.g., Carruthers and Stege 2013).…”
Section: How To Study Humanly Optimal Algorithmsmentioning
confidence: 99%