2012
DOI: 10.3758/s13423-012-0278-y
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On the discrepant results in synchrony judgment and temporal-order judgment tasks: a quantitative model

Abstract: Research on the perception of temporal order uses either temporal-order judgment (TOJ) tasks or synchrony judgment (SJ) tasks, in both of which two stimuli are presented with some temporal delay and observers must judge the order of presentation. Results generally differ across tasks, raising concerns about whether they measure the same processes. We present a model including sensory and decisional parameters that places these tasks in a common framework that allows studying their implications on observed perf… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

7
140
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 96 publications
(148 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
7
140
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The same is true about just-noticeable differences or other performance measures extracted from SJ and TOJ data, although these additional measures have not been discussed here (García-Pérez & Alcalá-Quintana, 2012a). These empirically observable and undeniable facts reflect a mixture of timing, decisional, and response processes that can only be separated out by fitting model-based psychometric functions with parameters that capture these influences.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The same is true about just-noticeable differences or other performance measures extracted from SJ and TOJ data, although these additional measures have not been discussed here (García-Pérez & Alcalá-Quintana, 2012a). These empirically observable and undeniable facts reflect a mixture of timing, decisional, and response processes that can only be separated out by fitting model-based psychometric functions with parameters that capture these influences.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…2b). The IC model just presented must be amended to cover realistically the mapping of judgments onto responses, a process during which errors can be made (see García-Pérez & Alcalá-Quintana, 2012a, 2012b. With this extension, the final form of the psychometric functions in SJ and TOJ tasks are…”
Section: Independent-channels Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The net effect is that the estimated perceptual midpoint is artifactually shifted in either direction by a meaningful, and potentially large, amount. Such response bias , defined as unbalanced “left”/“right” responses when undecided, 1 produces analogous shifts in many other psychophysical tasks (see, e.g., Alcalá-Quintana & García-Pérez, 2011; García-Pérez & Alcalá-Quintana, 2010a, 2010b, 2011a, 2011b, 2012a, 2012b), and other accounts describe shifts due to what we will define later as decisional bias (see, e.g., Schneider, 2011; Schneider & Komlos, 2008). These findings cast doubts on the validity of estimates of the perceptual midpoint obtained with the landmark task.…”
mentioning
confidence: 95%
“…However, especially in trials with very short SOAs, perceptually undecided participants could be inclined to respond with the location that was most salient in the experiment, namely the location in which they expected pain. A shift in PSS values could then reflect a bias in the decision process, rather than a genuinely perceptual effect of pain anticipation (for a more extensive discussion of response bias in TOJ tasks, see García-Pérez & Alcalá-Quintana, 2012;Spence & Parise, 2010). Filbrich et al (2016) argued that it is likely that the instructions used in our studies (Vanden Bulcke et al, 2013 have strongly induced such response bias.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%