2017
DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2017.1309437
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Optimal testing time for suppression of competitors during interference resolution

Abstract: Interference between competing memory traces is a common cause of memory failure. Recent research has demonstrated a suppression mechanism that operates at retrieval to resolve interference. Using an adaptation of the suppression paradigm in Healey, Ngo, and Hasher [(2014). Below-baseline suppression of competitors during interference resolution by younger but not older adults. Psychological Science, 25(1), 145-151. doi: 10.1177/0956797613501169 ], we tested whether the ability to suppress competing memory tra… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

6
14
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
1
1

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
6
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We speculate that states of lower inhibition at non-optimal times of day (the morning for most young adults; Hasher et al, 1999;May, 1998;May & Hasher, 2017;Yoon et al, 1999) might highlight relatively weak connections between memories, facilitating both generalization and false memory. This is consistent with previous work demonstrating that at non-optimal times of day there is both an increased ability to find remote connections between words (Simor & Polner, 2017;Wieth & Zacks, 2011) and increased memory interference (Ngo & Hasher, 2017).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…We speculate that states of lower inhibition at non-optimal times of day (the morning for most young adults; Hasher et al, 1999;May, 1998;May & Hasher, 2017;Yoon et al, 1999) might highlight relatively weak connections between memories, facilitating both generalization and false memory. This is consistent with previous work demonstrating that at non-optimal times of day there is both an increased ability to find remote connections between words (Simor & Polner, 2017;Wieth & Zacks, 2011) and increased memory interference (Ngo & Hasher, 2017).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…A major caveat to this finding is that our sample consisted predominantly of neutral chronotypes ( Supplementary Table 3), whereas studies reporting chronotype-related effects typically sample more densely from extreme morning and evening types (May, 1998;May et al, 2005;Simor & Polner, 2017;Wieth & Zacks, 2011). Though neutral type young adults often fail to show time of day effects (May & Hasher, 2017), morning testing has been classified as a non-optimal time for both evening and neutral type young adults (Ngo et al, 2018;Ngo & Hasher, 2017), so it is likely the morning constituted a non-optimal time for the vast majority of our sample (we had very few morning types). To examine whether an evening benefit could emerge in this task, future work with strong morning types, which could be achieved by sampling older adults (May et al, 2005;Yoon et al, 1999), would be informative.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…However, there are at least two circumstances under which young adults’ cognitive control has been reported to be reduced: (1) when in a positive mood (e.g., Biss, Hasher, & Thomas, 2010; Fredrickson, 2001; Schmitz, De Rosa, & Anderson, 2009; Vermeulen, 2010) and (2) when tested at off-peak times of day. With respect to time of day, there is substantial evidence that young adults operating at a time that is desynchronised with their circadian preference show reduced control relative to others operating at a peak time (e.g., Hornik & Tal, 2009; Intons-Peterson, Rocchi, West, McLellan, & Hackney, 1999; Lara, Madrid, & Correa, 2014; Lehmann, Marks, & Hanstock, 2013; Ngo & Hasher, 2016; Pica, Pierro, & Kruglanski, 2014; Ramírez, García, & Valdez, 2012; Ramírez et al, 2006; Webb, 1982). For example, in a study by Rowe and colleagues (2006) using a 1-back task on pictures with superimposed distracting words, evening chronotype young adults showed priming for the irrelevant words when tested in the morning, but not when tested in the afternoon.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%