1999
DOI: 10.1006/obhd.1999.2847
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Overconfidence: It Depends on How, What, and Whom You Ask

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

38
396
1
8

Year Published

2003
2003
2009
2009

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 663 publications
(443 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
38
396
1
8
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, when judges are 90 to 99% sure in their answer to a binary choice question, they are typically correct about 70% of the time; when they are only 50% sure, they are correct around 55% of the time. The results for interval estimates have been similar, though perhaps stronger: For example, judges' 90% intervals typically contain the correct answer less than 50% of the time (Klayman et al, 1999;Russo & Schoemaker, 1992). (The hard-easy effect has not been investigated for interval estimates.)…”
Section: Overconfidence In Interval Estimatesmentioning
confidence: 94%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…For example, when judges are 90 to 99% sure in their answer to a binary choice question, they are typically correct about 70% of the time; when they are only 50% sure, they are correct around 55% of the time. The results for interval estimates have been similar, though perhaps stronger: For example, judges' 90% intervals typically contain the correct answer less than 50% of the time (Klayman et al, 1999;Russo & Schoemaker, 1992). (The hard-easy effect has not been investigated for interval estimates.)…”
Section: Overconfidence In Interval Estimatesmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…That is, when confidence is high, it's generally too high, and when it's low, it's generally too low-the typical pattern of miscalibration. Similarly, when accuracy is low for a set of questions it's likely to be lower than the available information would lead one to expect, and when accuracy is high it's likely to be higher than one would expect (Dawes & Mulford, 1996;Klayman et al, 1999;Suantak, Bolger, & Ferrell, 1996). This leads to overconfidence for "hard" question sets, and occasionally even underconfidence for "easy" question sets -the hardeasy effect.…”
Section: Overconfidence In Interval Estimatesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…and then have participants estimate 90% confidence intervals around their answers. Results show that these confidence intervals are too narrow, suggesting that people are too sure they know the correct answer; 90% confidence intervals contain the correct answer less than 50% of the time (Alpert & Raiffa, 1969Klayman, Soll, Gonzalez-Vallejo, & Barlas, 1999;Soll & Klayman, 2004).…”
Section: Trouble With Overconfidencementioning
confidence: 96%