2005
DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.3.502
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Parafoveal Semantic Processing of Emotional Visual Scenes.

Abstract: The authors investigated whether emotional pictorial stimuli are especially likely to be processed in parafoveal vision. Pairs of emotional and neutral visual scenes were presented parafoveally (2.1°or 2.5°o f visual angle from a central fixation point) for 150 -3,000 ms, followed by an immediate recognition test (500-ms delay). Results indicated that (a) the first fixation was more likely to be placed onto the emotional than the neutral scene; (b) recognition sensitivity (AЈ) was generally higher for the emot… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

10
76
2

Year Published

2008
2008
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 82 publications
(88 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
10
76
2
Order By: Relevance
“…A tentative conclusion is that affective significance is assessed through overt attention (Avero & Calvo, 2006), but not through covert attention (present study). However, this conclusion would be at odds with prior research that has found either facilitated orienting to emotional pictures (as shown by eye movements; Calvo & Lang, 2004;Nummenmaa et al, 2006) or (Calvo & Lang, 2005) when the pictures were presented parafoveally. A way to account for these discrepancies involves examining the role of prior identification of the stimuli as a prerequisite for affective processing.…”
Section: Resultscontrasting
confidence: 54%
“…A tentative conclusion is that affective significance is assessed through overt attention (Avero & Calvo, 2006), but not through covert attention (present study). However, this conclusion would be at odds with prior research that has found either facilitated orienting to emotional pictures (as shown by eye movements; Calvo & Lang, 2004;Nummenmaa et al, 2006) or (Calvo & Lang, 2005) when the pictures were presented parafoveally. A way to account for these discrepancies involves examining the role of prior identification of the stimuli as a prerequisite for affective processing.…”
Section: Resultscontrasting
confidence: 54%
“…First, a trial could not be included if the target change was not correctly signaled by a button press or was not correctly named at the end of the trial's motion phase, because in those trials attention was not on the correct target. Second, trials were excluded from further analysis if, at event start, the distance of the gaze vector to the tobe-changed target was less than 5°-that is, within the range of parafoveal vision (Calvo & Lang, 2005). The reason for this exclusion was that in those trials the participants could have focused on the particular target just by chance, so that the lack of a saccade should be considered as accidental and not be interpreted as resulting from a certain gaze-control strategy.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The mean luminance and colour saturation (for red, green and blue) for each picture were measured and some pictures were adjusted to maintain homogeneity between unpleasant and neutral sets (Adobe Photoshop Ò software, see [25,34]). Separate analyses of variance (ANOVA) performed on the mean luminance, the colour saturation and the standard deviation of luminance values (i.e.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, Calvo and Lang [25] explored parafoveal vision at 10°of eccentricity and evidenced a preferential gaze attraction for emotional pictures when compared with neutral ones. At the same eccentricity, they also showed that emotional pictures were better remembered, even with brief presentations (150 ms).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation