1983
DOI: 10.2307/1129704
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Paralinguistic Features of Adult Speech to Infants and Small Children

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

1989
1989
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 91 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Initially termed 'baby talk' (Ferguson, 1964), later 'motherese', and then 'parentese' (Fernald, 1985;Fernald et al, 1989;Grieser & Kuhl, 1988), this speech register is distinguished from adult-directed speech (ADS) by a variety of segmental and prosodic features, including higher overall pitch and wider pitch range, slowed speech rate, exaggerated intonation contours, fewer and simpler lexical items, shorter utterances, and longer pauses between phrases (Cooper & Aslin, 1990;Fernald, 1985;Fernald et al, 1989;Fernald & Simon, 1984;Garnica, 1977;Grieser & Kuhl, 1988;Stern et al, 1983;Tang & Maidment, 1996). Infant-directed speech is used across cultures in spoken and signed languages by parents, grandparents, siblings, teachers, and adults who do not have their own children (Ferguson, 1964;Jacobson et al, 1983;Kuhl et al, 1997;Reilly & Bellugi, 1996). Across different languages, similarities and differences have been observed in specific linguistic aspects of IDS (Broesch & Bryant, 2015;Han et al, 2020;Narayan & McDermott, 2016).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Initially termed 'baby talk' (Ferguson, 1964), later 'motherese', and then 'parentese' (Fernald, 1985;Fernald et al, 1989;Grieser & Kuhl, 1988), this speech register is distinguished from adult-directed speech (ADS) by a variety of segmental and prosodic features, including higher overall pitch and wider pitch range, slowed speech rate, exaggerated intonation contours, fewer and simpler lexical items, shorter utterances, and longer pauses between phrases (Cooper & Aslin, 1990;Fernald, 1985;Fernald et al, 1989;Fernald & Simon, 1984;Garnica, 1977;Grieser & Kuhl, 1988;Stern et al, 1983;Tang & Maidment, 1996). Infant-directed speech is used across cultures in spoken and signed languages by parents, grandparents, siblings, teachers, and adults who do not have their own children (Ferguson, 1964;Jacobson et al, 1983;Kuhl et al, 1997;Reilly & Bellugi, 1996). Across different languages, similarities and differences have been observed in specific linguistic aspects of IDS (Broesch & Bryant, 2015;Han et al, 2020;Narayan & McDermott, 2016).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Mothers across cultures speak and sing to their pre-verbal infants in the course of providing care (Fernald, 1992; Trehub and Trainor, 1998; Dissanayake, 2000; Trehub, 2000). Their manner of speaking or singing to infants (infant-directed or ID) differs dramatically from their manner in other contexts (adult-directed or AD; self-directed or non-ID)(Ferguson, 1964; Jacobson et al, 1983; Fernald and Simon, 1984; Trainor et al, 1997; Trehub et al, 1997a,b), with notable variations across cultures (Grieser and Kuhl, 1988; Fernald et al, 1989; Kitamura et al, 2002). In general, ID speech features higher pitch, expanded pitch contours, slower speaking rate, longer vowels, larger dynamic range, and greater rhythmicity and repetition than AD speech (Stern et al, 1982, 1983; Fernald and Simon, 1984; Fernald et al, 1989).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children's uncertainty about questions and statements in the region of the category boundary is consistent with their lesser accuracy than adults in identifying questions from full, unaltered utterances (Saindon et al, 2016) and their considerably lesser skill in using preterminal cues to utterance type (Saindon et al, 2017). Because children are more successful at identifying questions and statements in child-directed than in adult-directed utterances (Saindon et al, 2017), the former featuring exaggerated prosodic cues (Foulkes et al, 2005;Jacobson et al, 1983), it is possible that their identification functions for child-directed utterances would be steeper and more adultlike than those observed in the present study. Note, however, that children's perception of emotional prosody exhibits a very protracted course of development, with 13-yr-olds performing below adult levels (Aguert et al, 2013).…”
Section: Discusssion and Conclusionmentioning
confidence: 78%