2003
DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.3.521
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Patterns of knowledge in children's addition.

Abstract: Patterns of conceptual and procedural knowledge of addition were examined in 5- to 8-year-olds (N = 80). Conceptual knowledge was measured by assessing children's responses to problems in which addends were reordered or decomposed and recombined. Problems were presented using abstract symbols, numbers, and physical objects. Children were more successful in noticing that addends had been reordered rather than decomposed and in noticing the decomposition of addends presented with objects rather than with symbols… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
77
0
2

Year Published

2007
2007
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 76 publications
(91 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
(111 reference statements)
9
77
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Nevertheless, it does not fit the general claim that with experience, children become faster and more accurate at solving addition problems and also tend to use more sophisticated strategies, such as order-irrelevant, decomposition, and retrieval strategies (Baroody et al, 1983;Canobi et al, 1998Canobi et al, , 20022003;Geary, Brown & Samaranayake, 1991;Goldman, Mertz & Pellegrino, 1989;Resnick, 1992;Rittle-Johnson & Siegler, 1998;Siegler, 1987;but see, McNeil, 2007;Robinson & Dubé, 2009;Robinson & Ninowski, 2003;Torbeyns et al, 2009). It also seems to contradict the results of Baroody et al (1983), which show that approximately 80% of their third graders applied the commutativity-based shortcut to solve arithmetic problems (see also, Canobi et al, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 52%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Nevertheless, it does not fit the general claim that with experience, children become faster and more accurate at solving addition problems and also tend to use more sophisticated strategies, such as order-irrelevant, decomposition, and retrieval strategies (Baroody et al, 1983;Canobi et al, 1998Canobi et al, , 20022003;Geary, Brown & Samaranayake, 1991;Goldman, Mertz & Pellegrino, 1989;Resnick, 1992;Rittle-Johnson & Siegler, 1998;Siegler, 1987;but see, McNeil, 2007;Robinson & Dubé, 2009;Robinson & Ninowski, 2003;Torbeyns et al, 2009). It also seems to contradict the results of Baroody et al (1983), which show that approximately 80% of their third graders applied the commutativity-based shortcut to solve arithmetic problems (see also, Canobi et al, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 52%
“…One frequently used approach to measure procedural knowledge is to ask children to solve addition problems and afterwards have them explain their strategies (e.g., Baroody & Gannon, 1984;Baroody, Ginsburg & Waxman, 1983;Bisanz & LeFevre, 1992;Canobi et al, 1998Canobi et al, , 2002Canobi et al, , 2003Cowan & Renton, 1996). Conceptual knowledge in these studies has, for example, been assessed by letting children observe a puppet solving problem pairs (see, e.g.…”
Section: 1mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Despite strong relations between schoolchildren's conceptual understanding and their reported problem-solving procedures and accuracy (Canobi, 2004(Canobi, , 2005Canobi et al, 1998Canobi et al, , 2003, the nature of concept-procedure interactions in basic addition and subtraction is not yet well understood. For example, cross-sectional research indicates that children who correctly apply concepts such as inversion and commutativity to related problems solve randomly ordered problems with greater accuracy than do other children (Canobi, 2004(Canobi, , 2005Canobi et al, 1998Canobi et al, , 2003.…”
Section: Exploring Concept-procedures Interactionsmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…These levels follow a progressive order of comprehension in the child (Canobi, Reeve, & Pattison, 2003;Fuson, Smith, & LoCicero, 1997). This was confirmed by Maccini and Hughes (2000) with a teaching sequence of three levels: concrete (manipulation of physical objects), semi-concrete (representation with drawings), and abstract (use of mathematical symbols).…”
mentioning
confidence: 70%