2019
DOI: 10.1038/s41562-019-0707-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

People prefer coordinated punishment in cooperative interactions

Abstract: Human groups can often maintain high levels of cooperation despite the threat of exploitation by individuals who reap the benefits of cooperation without contributing to its costs [1][2][3][4] . Prominent theoretical models suggest that cooperation is particularly likely to thrive if people join forces to curb free riding and punish their non-contributing peers in a coordinated fashion 5 . However, it is unclear whether and, if so, how people actually condition their punishment of peers on punishment behaviour… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

0
39
0
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 61 publications
(40 citation statements)
references
References 92 publications
0
39
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Furthermore, alternative forms of punishment such as gossip or ostracism have been discussed as well (see Guala, 2012 for ethnographic record andMolleman et al, 2019 for coordinated punishment). However, despite the disagreements over what exactly are the underlying motives, costly punishment has been identified as an important phenomenon in the study of human cooperation because it addresses the issue of how to respond to acts of free-riding (Balliet et al, 2011;Boyd, Gintis, Bowles, & Richerson, 2003;Gurerk, Irlenbusch, & Rockenbach, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, alternative forms of punishment such as gossip or ostracism have been discussed as well (see Guala, 2012 for ethnographic record andMolleman et al, 2019 for coordinated punishment). However, despite the disagreements over what exactly are the underlying motives, costly punishment has been identified as an important phenomenon in the study of human cooperation because it addresses the issue of how to respond to acts of free-riding (Balliet et al, 2011;Boyd, Gintis, Bowles, & Richerson, 2003;Gurerk, Irlenbusch, & Rockenbach, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…People can also punish because of general conformity (if others punish, so should I) or adherence to the fairness norm (if others pay costs of punishment, so should I). These factors lead to conditional punishment based on expectation that others will punish as well (Kamei, 2018;Molleman et al, 2019). There is also 'false enforcement' (Centola et al, 2005) when people enforce unpopular norms to show that they have complied out of genuine conviction and not because of social pressure.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These results differ from those predicted by the bystander effect studies and Carpenter et al (2012), who find that punishment levels are amongst the lowest in the CN and that more prosocial punishment is used in DCN (to maintain or raise contributions), compared to CN and UCN. A possible reason is that people may prefer coordinated punishment and may thus be willing to punish someone only when another individual is willing to do so as well (Ramalingam et al, 2016;Molleman et al, 2019). Another related reason may be that individuals may act as 'rational bystanders' if they believe that helping is only effective when more than one help-giver pitches in (Greitemeyer and Mügge, 2013) or that their personal contribution will be superfluous (Krueger and Massey, 2009); this is not unlikely in more complex environments like the nonlinear CPR appropriation dilemma.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%