2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2012.02.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Phonetic richness can outweigh prosodically-driven phonological knowledge when learning words in an artificial language

Abstract: a b s t r a c tHow do Dutch and Korean listeners use acoustic-phonetic information when learning words in an artificial language? Dutch has a voiceless 'unaspirated' stop, produced with shortened Voice Onset Time (VOT) in prosodic strengthening environments (e.g., in domain-initial position and under prominence), enhancing the feature { À spread glottis}; Korean has a voiceless 'aspirated' stop produced with lengthened VOT in similar environments, enhancing the feature {þ spread glottis}. Given this crosslingu… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It might be suggested that this happens because pitch marks the edges of the structural units of speech cross-linguistically, while the perception of durational cues is driven by the native phonology of the participants. When adults attend to an unfamiliar language, universal phonetically rich cues can override phonological knowledge specific to their native language (see, e.g., Kim, Cho, & McQueen, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It might be suggested that this happens because pitch marks the edges of the structural units of speech cross-linguistically, while the perception of durational cues is driven by the native phonology of the participants. When adults attend to an unfamiliar language, universal phonetically rich cues can override phonological knowledge specific to their native language (see, e.g., Kim, Cho, & McQueen, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The results revealed that the presence of DIS in the onset of the post-boundary word (e.g., company), even in the absence of preboundary lengthening of the preceding word (e.g., mill) serves as a cue to lexical segmentation via resolving lexical ambiguity that arises temporally at the juncture (milk is a competitor of mill in the mill#company sequence). This result implies that the fine-grained phonetic detail of DIS even in the absence of other prosodic cues to the boundary is exploited by listeners in speech comprehension, warranting further studies that explore roles of DIS in various other aspects of speech comprehension across languages (e.g., Kim, Cho, and McQueen 2012).…”
Section: Syntagmatic and Paradigmatic Contrast Enhancementmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…The characterisation of this segmentation process thus involves determining the role of sublexical cues and the progressive involvement of lexical information. At least four types of sublexical cues have been investigated in word learning experiments that include statistical information, phonotactic regularities (Finn & Hudson Kam, 2008), fine-grained acoustic-phonetic regularities (Fernandes, Ventura, & Kolinsky, 2007; Kim, Cho, & McQueen, 2012) and prosodic regularities, particularly rhythmic regularities (e.g., Saffran, Newport, & Aslin, 1996).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%