2002
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.28.3.572
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Phonological effects in visual word recognition: Investigating the impact of feedback activation.

Abstract: P. M. Pexman, S. J. Lupker, and D. Jared (2001) reported longer response latencies in lexical decision tasks (LDTs) for homophones (e.g., maid) than for nonhomophones, and attributed this homophone effect to orthographic competition created by feedback activation from phonology. In the current study, two predictions of this feedback account were tested: (a) In LDT, observe homophone effects should be observed but not regularity or homograph effects because most exception words (e.g., pint) and homographs (e.g.… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

10
54
0

Year Published

2005
2005
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 41 publications
(64 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
10
54
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This interpretation of the N200 appears consistent with data reported by Pexman, Lupker, and Reggin (2002) that suggest that homophone effects in lexical decisions are initially triggered by phonological processing. However, the Pexman et al (2002) study also indicated that homophone effects in lexical decision actually arise from competition in the orthographic system (i.e., the feedback inconsistency effect reported in Stone, Vanhoy, & Van Orden, 1997). Thus, the N200 effect may indicate the early operation of phonological and orthographic processes, rather than primarily a phonological effect.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 91%
“…This interpretation of the N200 appears consistent with data reported by Pexman, Lupker, and Reggin (2002) that suggest that homophone effects in lexical decisions are initially triggered by phonological processing. However, the Pexman et al (2002) study also indicated that homophone effects in lexical decision actually arise from competition in the orthographic system (i.e., the feedback inconsistency effect reported in Stone, Vanhoy, & Van Orden, 1997). Thus, the N200 effect may indicate the early operation of phonological and orthographic processes, rather than primarily a phonological effect.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 91%
“…If the mediated-priming effect found with orthographically mediated prime-target word pairs was due to semantic feedback's spreading to phonological representations, a mediated-priming effect also should have been found with homophonically mediated word pairs in which mediating (e.g., toad) and target (e.g., towed) words shared considerably more phonology. Furthermore, it is important to note that the absence of mediated priming with homophonically mediated word pairs could not have been due to a lack of phonological activation during target processing, given that the naming task explicitly requires the activation of phonological information (e.g., Frost et al, 2003;Pexman, Lupker, & Reggin, 2002).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As in previous studies that have utilized the mediated-priming paradigm (e.g., Farrar et al, 2001;Reimer, 2006;Reimer et al, 2001), planned comparisons were used to test for direct-and mediated-priming effects within each type of prime-target word pair. Given that the items used in the present study were not randomly selected, tests using items as the random variable were not conducted in the present study (Wike & Church, 1976; see also Pexman, Lupker, & Reggin, 2002).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The real words were chosen from a public access database (http://www.psych.rl.ac.uk/) based on a rating scale given for number of letters and word imagibility (range 550-700). Pseudowords were chosen from Pexman et al (2002). Concrete nouns were selected for the word stimuli.…”
Section: Visual Word Discrimination Taskmentioning
confidence: 99%