2019
DOI: 10.1037/dev0000738
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Point, walk, talk: Links between three early milestones, from observation and parental report.

Abstract: share first authorship. Elika Bergelson, Andrei Amatuni, and Shannon Dailey were in Brain and Cognitive Sciences at the University of Rochester during data collection. Authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
35
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
2

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
0
35
0
Order By: Relevance
“…When considering communicative gestures, direct links have been found between pointing and language, for both language comprehension and production (e.g., Colonnesi, Stams, Koster, & Noom, 2010; Iverson & Goldin‐Meadow, 2005; Rowe & Goldin‐Meadow, 2009; Salo, Reeb‐Sutherland, Frenkel, Bowman, & Rowe, 2019). For example, a longitudinal study of infants aged 6–18 months demonstrated that children who pointed had larger vocabularies than children who did not point, in both cross‐sectional and longitudinal observations (Moore, Dailey, Garrison, Amatuni, & Bergelson, 2019). Since more rudimentary communicative gestures (e.g., pointing, reaching for objects, nodding head “no”) are a predictor of later language (e.g., vocabulary; Bavin et al, 2008), they may serve as a foundation for and indicator of later communicative skills (Liszkowski, 2008).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When considering communicative gestures, direct links have been found between pointing and language, for both language comprehension and production (e.g., Colonnesi, Stams, Koster, & Noom, 2010; Iverson & Goldin‐Meadow, 2005; Rowe & Goldin‐Meadow, 2009; Salo, Reeb‐Sutherland, Frenkel, Bowman, & Rowe, 2019). For example, a longitudinal study of infants aged 6–18 months demonstrated that children who pointed had larger vocabularies than children who did not point, in both cross‐sectional and longitudinal observations (Moore, Dailey, Garrison, Amatuni, & Bergelson, 2019). Since more rudimentary communicative gestures (e.g., pointing, reaching for objects, nodding head “no”) are a predictor of later language (e.g., vocabulary; Bavin et al, 2008), they may serve as a foundation for and indicator of later communicative skills (Liszkowski, 2008).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One potential example of a mechanism for active information seeking in early word learning comes from research by Walle and Campos (2014), who reported links between the onset of walking and vocabulary growth (but cf. Moore, Dailey, Garrison, Amatuni, & Bergelson, 2019). A possible mechanism for this developmental connection is that walking children often use their greater ability to carry objects to bring their caregivers objects that interest them.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While none of the CPs included in our data were interpreted as words by our trained child phonology experts, it is possible that some CPs reflected early word production. In separate work assessing early milestones in this sample of infants (Moore et al, 2019), we find few infants produce their first word before 10 months (4 via observational data, 12 via parent report; parent vs. researcher criteria likely vary, especially for infants' very first word). However, even if infants were producing proto-words, rather than established babble consonants, our analysis relates this once again to VMS, since these vocalizations (whether babble or rudimentary words) were more often consistent when object labels contained consonants in infants' VMS repertoire.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 84%