Online abstract. One of the most astonishing features of human language is its capacity to convey information efficiently in context. Many theories provide informal accounts of communicative inference, yet there have been few successes in making precise, quantitative predictions about pragmatic reasoning. We examine judgments about simple referential communication games, modeling behavior in these games by assuming that speakers attempt to be informative, and that listeners use Bayesian inference to recover speakers' intended referents. Our model provides a close, parameter-free fit to human judgments, suggesting that using information-theoretic tools to predict pragmatic reasoning may lead to more effective formal models of communication.One of the most astonishing features of human language is its ability to convey information efficiently in context. Each utterance need not carry every detail; instead, listeners can infer speakers' intended meanings by assuming utterances convey only relevant information. These communicative inferences rely on the shared assumption that speakers are informative, but not more so than is necessary given the communicators' common knowledge and the task at hand. Many theories provide high-level accounts of these kinds of inferences (1-3), yet-perhaps be-1
English and Russian color terms divide the color spectrum differently. Unlike English, Russian makes an obligatory distinction between lighter blues (''goluboy'') and darker blues (''siniy''). We investigated whether this linguistic difference leads to differences in color discrimination. We tested English and Russian speakers in a speeded color discrimination task using blue stimuli that spanned the siniy/goluboy border. We found that Russian speakers were faster to discriminate two colors when they fell into different linguistic categories in Russian (one siniy and the other goluboy) than when they were from the same linguistic category (both siniy or both goluboy). Moreover, this category advantage was eliminated by a verbal, but not a spatial, dual task. These effects were stronger for difficult discriminations (i.e., when the colors were perceptually close) than for easy discriminations (i.e., when the colors were further apart). English speakers tested on the identical stimuli did not show a category advantage in any of the conditions. These results demonstrate that (i) categories in language affect performance on simple perceptual color tasks and (ii) the effect of language is online (and can be disrupted by verbal interference).categorization ͉ cross-linguistic ͉ Whorf
The MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories (CDIs) are a widely used family of parent-report instruments for easy and inexpensive data-gathering about early language acquisition. CDI data have been used to explore a variety of theoretically important topics, but, with few exceptions, researchers have had to rely on data collected in their own lab. In this paper, we remedy this issue by presenting Wordbank, a structured database of CDI data combined with a browsable web interface. Wordbank archives CDI data across languages and labs, providing a resource for researchers interested in early language, as well as a platform for novel analyses. The site allows interactive exploration of patterns of vocabulary growth at the level of both individual children and particular words. We also introduce wordbankr, a software package for connecting to the database directly. Together, these tools extend the abilities of students and researchers to explore quantitative trends in vocabulary development.
In simple tests of preference, infants as young as newborns prefer faces and face-like stimuli over distractors. Little is known, however, about the development of attention to faces in complex scenes. We recorded eye-movements of 3-, 6-, and 9-month-old infants and adults during free-viewing of clips from A Charlie Brown Christmas (an animated film). The tendency to look at faces increased with age. Using novel computational tools, we found that 3-month-olds were less consistent (across individuals) in where they looked than were older infants. Moreover, younger infants' fixations were best predicted by low-level image salience, rather than the locations of faces. Between 3 and 9 months of age, infants gradually focus their attention on faces. We discuss several possible interpretations of this shift in terms of social development, cross-modal integration, and attentional/executive control.A lot can be learned about our social world from the faces of others. Faces provide information about age, race, gender, physical health, emotional state, and focus of attention, giving observers a window into the mental states of other human beings. During the first year after birth, infants begin to extract a large amount of information from faces: they begin to recognize identities (Pascalis, De Haan, Nelson, & De Schonen, 1998), recognize and prefer faces from their own race , detect affect (Cohn & Tronick, 1983;Tronick, 1989), and follow gaze (Corkum & Moore, 1998;Scaife & Bruner, 1975). However, these sophisticated abilities are of little use if infants don't look at faces to begin with. Put another way, to extract information from faces, infants must first attend to them.Although there is a large literature on the origins and development of infants' face representations during infancy, far less research has examined the behavior of infants outside of controlled laboratory settings. In particular, both the extent to which infants attend to faces when other objects are present-as in most real-world situations-and the extent to which this behavior changes across development are still largely unknown. The reasons for this gap in the literature may be both methodological and theoretical. Methodologically, standard lookingtime paradigms used in infant research typically produce only qualitative evidence and do not make sense in older populations; hence it is difficult to design experimental paradigms whose results can be compared across wide age ranges. Theoretically, many researchers have been interested primarily in the question of innateness: whether human infants are predisposed to treat human faces as "special" relative to other objects and whether the representations underlying these judgments are qualitatively similar to those used by adults. Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the result...
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.