2018
DOI: 10.1017/s0022226718000336
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Polar answers

Abstract: How do people answer polar questions? In this fourteen-language study of answers to questions in conversation, we compare the two main strategies; first, interjection-type answers such asuh-huh(or equivalentsyes,mm, head nods, etc.), and second, repetition-type answers that repeat some or all of the question. We find that all languages offer both options, but that there is a strong asymmetry in their frequency of use, with a global preference for interjection-type answers. We propose that this preference is mo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
25
0
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 81 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
1
25
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…So turn-taking, the organization of sequences, the organization of repair mechanisms in talk, the conversational preference for particular actions -all part of that procedural infrastructure -are proving to be empirically robust across the structural variation and diversity of languages and language groups (see e.g. Clift, 2016;Enfield et al, 2017;Kendrick et al, 2014;. And, of course, such cross-linguistic analysis is also able to identify the extent to which specific practices may be universal.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…So turn-taking, the organization of sequences, the organization of repair mechanisms in talk, the conversational preference for particular actions -all part of that procedural infrastructure -are proving to be empirically robust across the structural variation and diversity of languages and language groups (see e.g. Clift, 2016;Enfield et al, 2017;Kendrick et al, 2014;. And, of course, such cross-linguistic analysis is also able to identify the extent to which specific practices may be universal.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Nonetheless, research in CA takes as its point of departure Sacks' (1984: 22) fundamental assumption that there is 'order at all points', and in so doing has produced countless studies -both qualitative and quantitative -that robustly demonstrate not only the existence, but also the systematic deployment of various different sorts of agreement and confirmation cross-linguistically (see e.g. Bolden, 2016;Enfield et al, 2017;Raymond, 2003;Raymond, 2015;.…”
Section: On 'Actual Produced and Constitutive Detail' In Practice: mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…To verify Li and Thompson’s (1981) observation, I examined the 196 polar answers in my data following the framework of the recent cross-linguistic works on question-answer system (Enfield et al, 2019; Stivers, 2018; Stivers and Enfield, 2010). They categorize polar responses into the following three types: (1) interjection type , which does not assert a proposition in and of itself but does confirm or disconfirm one; (2) repetition type , which repeats fully or partially the elements without qualitative semantic alternation; and (3) other , which mainly includes transformative answers (Stivers and Hayashi, 2010).…”
Section: Grammatical Conformity In Mandarinmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…In contrast to a language like English, in which responses to polar questions normally employ polar response particles such as yes and no (Raymond, 2003), in Tzeltal an affirmative response is normally accomplished by repetition (Brown, 2010;Enfield et al, 2019). As a result, repetitional receipt sequences in Tzeltal typically consist of two repetitions, one as a receipt of the base second pair-part and one as a confirmation of the receipt.…”
Section: Non-minimal Post-expansionsmentioning
confidence: 99%