2006
DOI: 10.1007/s11031-006-9021-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Positive mood broadens visual attention to positive stimuli

Abstract: In an attempt to investigate the impact of positive emotions on visual attention within the context of Fredrickson's (1998) broaden-and-build model, eye tracking was used in two studies to measure visual attentional preferences of college students (n=58, n=26) to emotional pictures. Half of each sample experienced induced positive mood immediately before viewing slides of three similarlyvalenced images, in varying central-peripheral arrays. Attentional breadth was determined by measuring the percentage viewing… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

24
194
2
4

Year Published

2009
2009
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8
1
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 268 publications
(224 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
24
194
2
4
Order By: Relevance
“…This process may also underlie the finding with eye-tracking that positive affect promotes increased attention to peripheral visual stimuli (Wadlinger & Isaacowitz, 2006).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…This process may also underlie the finding with eye-tracking that positive affect promotes increased attention to peripheral visual stimuli (Wadlinger & Isaacowitz, 2006).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…First, we investigate the effect of emotional stimuli on the processing of a displays' context (using a contextual cueing task) and second, we investigate the effect of emotional stimuli on response times using an inefficient visual search task. Taking the first point, previous research has suggested that negative emotions lead to diminished processing of peripheral items and a narrowing of attention (Easterbrook, 1959, Fenske and Eastwood, 2003, Fredrickson, 1998, Wadlinger & Isaacowitz, 2006, Wells & Matthews, 1994. This focussing or narrowing of attention could occur in a number of situations including inducing social stress (Sanders, Baron & Moore, 1978, Huguet et al, 1999, creating ego threatening and time pressured situations (Chajut & Algom, 2003), threatening participants with electric shocks (Watchel, 1968) and presenting people with faces demonstrating negative emotions (Fenske & Eastwood, 2003).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This parallel may be somewhat exaggerated, however, as emotion and reward are sometimes experimentally operationalized similarly, and thus would produce similar effects in behavior. Specifically, both emotion and reward are often studied using shocks (Bauch et al, 2014;Bisby & Burgess, 2014;Dunsmoor et al, 2015;Jensen et al, 2007;Murty et al, 2012Murty et al, , 2011Pessoa, 2009;Phelps & LeDoux, 2005;Redondo et al, 2014;Schmidt et al, 2015;Wang et al, 2013;Weiner & Walker, 1966), food (Beaver et al, 2006;de Water et al, 2017;Isen & Geva, 1987;LaBar et al, 2001;Polanía et al, 2015;Talmi et al, 2013;Wadlinger & Isaacowitz, 2006), emotional face pictures (Bradley et al, 1997;Lin et al, 2012;Tsukiura & Cabeza, 2008;Vrijsen et al, 2013;Vuilleumier & Schwartz, 2001;Woud et al, 2013), or erotic/sexual pictures (Attard-Johnson & Bindemann, 2017;Bradley et al, 2001;Ferrey et al, 2012;Hamann et al, 2004;Iigaya et al, 2016;Most et al, 2007;Sescousse et al, 2013aSescousse et al, , 2010. As such, it would be expected that both emotion and reward demonstrate similar effects on cognition, as they can be studied using nearly identical experimental designs.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%