2008
DOI: 10.1163/156853908786131324
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Post-conflict non-affiliative behavioural strategies and subsequent social interaction in preschool boys with language impairment in comparison to preschool boys with typical language skills

Abstract: Conflicts between 20 boys (4-6 years old) with typical language development (TL) and between 11 boys (4-7 years old) with Language Impairment (LI) were examined in naturalistic preschool settings. Post-conflict aggression, passive withdrawal (auto-manipulation/irrelevant vocalizations) and active withdrawal (leaving conflict scene) were examined in relation to preceding behavioural situations and the likelihood of social interaction after conflict management. The boys with TL tended to display aggression to a … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
2

Relationship

0
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Further, data from observations show that it is not the frequency or length of utterance that matter to peers, but rather the poor social use of language and low linguistic sophistication make a difference (DeKroon et al., 2002; Salmenlinna & Laksoo, 2020). These findings are backed up by self-reports, task-based and observation studies, confirming the breadth of vocabulary knowledge as minimally related to how successful children with DLD are at using language to reconcile peer conflict (Bakopoulou & Dockrell, 2016; Horowitz et al., 2005, 2008; Marton et al., 2005). It is important therefore that researchers acknowledge that in the context of the SAM, ‘verbal resources’ includes not only syntactic and semantic language but also social and linguistic pragmatics.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 78%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Further, data from observations show that it is not the frequency or length of utterance that matter to peers, but rather the poor social use of language and low linguistic sophistication make a difference (DeKroon et al., 2002; Salmenlinna & Laksoo, 2020). These findings are backed up by self-reports, task-based and observation studies, confirming the breadth of vocabulary knowledge as minimally related to how successful children with DLD are at using language to reconcile peer conflict (Bakopoulou & Dockrell, 2016; Horowitz et al., 2005, 2008; Marton et al., 2005). It is important therefore that researchers acknowledge that in the context of the SAM, ‘verbal resources’ includes not only syntactic and semantic language but also social and linguistic pragmatics.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 78%
“…Regarding the good practice of participatory studies with children, the reported observation studies did not specify these aspects of their research. In the naturalistic observation category, only Horowitz et al.’s (2005, 2006, 2008) mentioned obtaining informed consent from parents and children themselves. Perhaps the use of filming might have prompted researchers to report on this ethical aspect of the study.…”
Section: Observation Studiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…It is unsurprising that peer conflict resolution would be harder for children with poor expressive language skills. To successfully repair conflict situations, children must negotiate, assert their position in a way which is not aggressive and should not surrender their own boundaries (Horowitz et al., 2008). The frequent peer conflicts children with LDs are observed to have are likely exacerbated if children misinterpret the intentions of their peers in the situation leading up to the conflict.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%