2015
DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12558
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Preparing for (valenced) action: The role of differential effort in the orthogonalized go/no‐go task

Abstract: Associating reward to task performance has been shown to benefit scores of cognitive functions. Importantly, this typically entails associating reward to the execution of a response, hence intertwining action-related processes with motivational ones. However, recently, preparatory action requirements (go/no-go) and outcome valence (reward/ punishment) were elegantly separated using a cued orthogonalized go/no-go task. Functional magnetic resonance imaging results from this task showed that typical areas of the… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

4
16
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 101 publications
(186 reference statements)
4
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Lavallee et al (2014) observed increased delta power over posterior electrodes during proactive control, presumably reflecting activity of a posterior attentional network. Finally, our results dovetail with those of Schevernels et al (2016), who reported a higher N1 to task-relevant go-targets compared to task-irrelevant nogo-targets.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Lavallee et al (2014) observed increased delta power over posterior electrodes during proactive control, presumably reflecting activity of a posterior attentional network. Finally, our results dovetail with those of Schevernels et al (2016), who reported a higher N1 to task-relevant go-targets compared to task-irrelevant nogo-targets.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…This slow negative potential with its maximum over central regions, is the most common ERP component when studying preparatory (i.e., proactive) cognitive processes. Schevernels et al (2016) reported in a cued go/nogo-study that in cued nogo- compared to cued go-trials the CNV was reduced, speaking for less employment of attentional resources. Here, we expected proactive motor control to be accompanied by increased activity in attentional networks, reflected in changes in occipital alpha, the CNV and the P1/N1-complex, as ERP correlate of early visual processing in extrastriate cortex (Hillyard and Anllo-Vento, 1998).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a series of studies, Guitart-Masip and colleagues found valence-action biases in an incentive Go/NoGo task, in that participants' performance was improved in trials with compatible ('natural') mappings, i.e., Go-Win, NoGo-Avoid Losing (Guitart-Masip et al, 2011Richter et al, 2014). In contrast, other recent studies manipulating incentives in Go/NoGo, Stop-signal, or approach/avoidance tasks failed to provide clear evidence for such valence-action biases (Boehler, Hopf, Stoppel, & Krebs, 2012;Schevernels, Bombeke, Krebs, & Boehler, 2016;Verbruggen & McLaren, 2016). This inconsistency probably results from paradigmatic differences.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Specifically, while the paradigm applied by colleagues (2011, 2012) contrasted win and loss incentives directly (and exclusively), more recent studies included no-incentive conditions as well. This was either done by adding no-incentive trials (Schevernels et al, 2016), by comparing win and loss trials to a no-incentive baseline in discrete groups (Hoofs et al, 2019), or by contrasting win, loss, and, no-incentive manipulations between groups (Verbruggen & McLaren, 2016). Consequentially, some of these studies did not feature a direct contrast between positive and negative valence.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation