Cognitive Neuroscience of Natural Language Use 2015
DOI: 10.1017/cbo9781107323667.008
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Putting Broca’s region into context: fMRI evidence for a role in predictive language processing

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
11
0
2

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

5
0

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 79 publications
(71 reference statements)
0
11
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Broca's region (Broca, 1861) is located in the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) and is usually defined as consisting of Brodmann area 44 and 45 (Amunts & Zilles, 2012). It is known to play a key role in speech production as well as in the processing of language input (e.g., see Kristensen & Wallentin, 2015 for a review). The exact function of the region, however, is disputed (Grodzinsky & Santi, 2008;Novick, Trueswell, & Thompson-Schill, 2010).…”
Section: Broca's Regionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Broca's region (Broca, 1861) is located in the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) and is usually defined as consisting of Brodmann area 44 and 45 (Amunts & Zilles, 2012). It is known to play a key role in speech production as well as in the processing of language input (e.g., see Kristensen & Wallentin, 2015 for a review). The exact function of the region, however, is disputed (Grodzinsky & Santi, 2008;Novick, Trueswell, & Thompson-Schill, 2010).…”
Section: Broca's Regionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is a general finding across research on attention that unexpected events attract attention (Fritz, Elhilali, David, & Shamma, 2007;Parmentier, Turner, & Perez, 2014). The same is arguably the case for non-prototypical linguistic utterances (Delong, Troyer, & Kutas, 2014;Kristensen & Wallentin, 2015;Kuperberg & Jaeger, 2015). In the absence of a context, however, the listener is forced to come up with an interpretation for the unusual division based on episodic memory or world-knowledge, which may be a cognitively demanding task.…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Danish is a V2 language (similar to German and Swedish), meaning that the finite verb occurs in the second position of the clause, while both subject and object can occur in the first position (Kristensen, 2013;Kristensen & Wallentin, 2015), and thus become a focus of attention (see Table 1 for examples).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is a general finding across research on attention, that unexpected and prominent events attract attention (Fritz, Elhilali, David, & Shamma, 2007;Parmentier, Turner, & Perez, 2014). The same is arguably the case for non-prototypical linguistic utterances (Delong, Troyer, & Kutas, 2014;Kristensen & Wallentin, 2015;Kuperberg & Jaeger, 2015). In the absence of a discourse context, the listener is forced to come up with an interpretation based on episodic memory or world-knowledge for why structure deviates from the default.…”
Section: Linguistic Allocation Of Attentionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…The experimental sentences were constructed using 11 Danish transitive verbs: "slår" [hits/smacks], "sparker" Verbs were chosen on the basis of being clearly imageable, having a simple agent-patient relationship with a default scenario unfolding in the horizontal plane. Sentences were created for each verb by combining it with case marked pronouns describing a male and a female character: Danish allows both subject-verb-object (SVO) and object-verb-subject (OVS) word order (Kristensen, 2013;Kristensen & Wallentin, 2015 2018, we decided that in order to prevent attentional fatigue, participants should not make more than eight drawings each. We therefore divided the sentences into 22 stimulus sets of 8 sentences.…”
Section: Stimuli and Materialsmentioning
confidence: 99%