“…In forensic science practice, moreover, crime-scene evidence is often ambiguous. The more ambiguous the evidence is, the harder it is to distinguish between deception and error (Whitman and Koppl (2009) help to explain why). Citing Schweitzer and Hsee (2002), Mazar and Ariely report, "participants are more likely to be dishonest as the private information they have becomes more ambiguous" (2006, p. 7).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As we have seen, the report says, "Forensic scientists who sit administratively in law enforcement agencies or prosecutors' offices, or who are hired by those units, are subject to a general risk of bias" (6-2). This statement may seem banal to some readers, but it contradicts the commonsense opinion of many forensic scientists (Whitman and Koppl 2009). Often, forensic scientists working in government crime labs genuinely believe that any forensic scientist working for defense counsel is venal, a "hired gun," and thus corrupt and untrustworthy.…”
Section: The Forensic-science Contextmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…In the context of forensic science, this message corresponds to a crime lab declaring, "match," "no match," or "inconclusive." (Whitman and Koppl (2009) defend this simplification.) The principal shape and the supplementary shape will sometimes be the same by chance.…”
Section: The Science Gamementioning
confidence: 93%
“…This sequencing is more important the more ambiguous is the crime-scene evidence. Whitman and Koppl (2009) argue that sequential unmasking is necessary but not sufficient to ensure unbiased testing "because the very act of submitting a sample conveys information." Their model supports the view that blind proficiency tests would help to reduce errors in forensic science.…”
Section: The Forensic-science Contextmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This fact very nearly follows from the fact that crime labs are typically organized under law enforcement agencies. Whitman and Koppl (2009) present a simple Bayesian model suggesting that the organization of forensic science may support a "rational" pro-prosecution bias. See Peart and Levy (2005) for a discussion of sympathy as a motive in economic theory.…”
“…In forensic science practice, moreover, crime-scene evidence is often ambiguous. The more ambiguous the evidence is, the harder it is to distinguish between deception and error (Whitman and Koppl (2009) help to explain why). Citing Schweitzer and Hsee (2002), Mazar and Ariely report, "participants are more likely to be dishonest as the private information they have becomes more ambiguous" (2006, p. 7).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As we have seen, the report says, "Forensic scientists who sit administratively in law enforcement agencies or prosecutors' offices, or who are hired by those units, are subject to a general risk of bias" (6-2). This statement may seem banal to some readers, but it contradicts the commonsense opinion of many forensic scientists (Whitman and Koppl 2009). Often, forensic scientists working in government crime labs genuinely believe that any forensic scientist working for defense counsel is venal, a "hired gun," and thus corrupt and untrustworthy.…”
Section: The Forensic-science Contextmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…In the context of forensic science, this message corresponds to a crime lab declaring, "match," "no match," or "inconclusive." (Whitman and Koppl (2009) defend this simplification.) The principal shape and the supplementary shape will sometimes be the same by chance.…”
Section: The Science Gamementioning
confidence: 93%
“…This sequencing is more important the more ambiguous is the crime-scene evidence. Whitman and Koppl (2009) argue that sequential unmasking is necessary but not sufficient to ensure unbiased testing "because the very act of submitting a sample conveys information." Their model supports the view that blind proficiency tests would help to reduce errors in forensic science.…”
Section: The Forensic-science Contextmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This fact very nearly follows from the fact that crime labs are typically organized under law enforcement agencies. Whitman and Koppl (2009) present a simple Bayesian model suggesting that the organization of forensic science may support a "rational" pro-prosecution bias. See Peart and Levy (2005) for a discussion of sympathy as a motive in economic theory.…”
Summary
Forensic examiners routinely compare a crime‐relevant mark of unknown origin against a single suspect's sample, which may create an expectation that the two will match. We tested how embedding the suspect's sample among known‐innocent fillers (i.e., an evidence lineup) affects expert decision‐making. Experienced fingerprint examiners (N = 43) compared crime‐relevant marks against either individual suspect fingerprints (i.e., the standard procedure) or arrays of fingerprints (i.e., evidence lineups), with a matching fingerprint either present or absent. Evidence lineups promoted conservative decision‐making, as evidenced by fewer correct IDs and a higher rate of inconclusive judgments. Though errors were rare, evidence lineups also occasionally revealed errors that would have otherwise gone undetected. Our findings thus support arguments that evidence lineups can expose fraud, identify flawed methodologies, and curb overconfidence. The potential benefits and challenges of implementing evidence lineups in forensic laboratories are discussed.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.