1977
DOI: 10.1016/0024-3841(77)90080-8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Relational grammar, ergativity, and Hindi-Urdu

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

1983
1983
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 2 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Hindi has a mixed agreement system -verb agreement morphology can index the features of the subject or the object or no argument depending on the sentence structure. The verb agrees with the subject if the subject lacks overt case-marking; failing that the verb agrees with the object if the object lacks overt case-marking; failing that the verb exhibits default agreement morphology (Kachru, 2006;Pandharipande & Kachru, 1977). In order to probe the hypothesis that agreement constitutes a necessary condition for local coherence to arise we manipulated agreement morphology on the post-RC verb for transitive sentences.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Hindi has a mixed agreement system -verb agreement morphology can index the features of the subject or the object or no argument depending on the sentence structure. The verb agrees with the subject if the subject lacks overt case-marking; failing that the verb agrees with the object if the object lacks overt case-marking; failing that the verb exhibits default agreement morphology (Kachru, 2006;Pandharipande & Kachru, 1977). In order to probe the hypothesis that agreement constitutes a necessary condition for local coherence to arise we manipulated agreement morphology on the post-RC verb for transitive sentences.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A general property of verbal agreement in Hindi is that it targets the hierarchically highest nonovertly case-marked nominal in the domain of the verb (Pandharipande and Kachru 1977). As a consequence, nominals may not agree if they carry an overt case marker like the ergative marker -ne or the accusative/dative marker -ko.…”
Section: Some Background On Hindi Ldamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a split-ergative language such as Hindi (Indo-European, spoken mainly in Northern India), agents of transitive actions do not receive uniform morphological marking (see Pandharipande & Kachru, 1977, and references cited therein). 2 In non-perfective contexts, A role arguments do not receive any overt marking (' nominative case'), whereas in perfective contexts, they are marked with the clitic ne (' ergative case') (examples (5), (6)) ; barring some lexical exceptions, S role arguments are typically null-marked (7) : 3 [2] The term ' split-ergative' is somewhat of a misnomer in Hindi since case-alternations on the subject are conditioned by perfectivity, case-alternations on the object are conditioned by animacy, definiteness, and specificity, and the two types of marking alternate (partly) independently of each other (Mohanan, 1994 The bifurcation of the notion of transitive agent on the basis of perfectivity in Hindi makes it a good case to test the hypothesis that the notion AGENT OF TRANSITIVE ACTION is innate and used to acquire case-marking.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a split-ergative language such as Hindi (Indo-European, spoken mainly in Northern India), agents of transitive actions do not receive uniform morphological marking (see Pandharipande & Kachru, 1977, and references cited therein). 2 In non-perfective contexts, A role arguments do not receive any overt marking (' nominative case'), whereas in perfective contexts, they are marked with the clitic ne (' ergative case') (examples (5), (6)) ; barring some lexical exceptions, S role arguments are typically null-marked (7) : 3 (5) wo haar uThaa-taa hae. '…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%