2011
DOI: 10.3758/s13420-011-0035-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Relationship between the rewarding and aversive effects of morphine and amphetamine in individual subjects

Abstract: Drugs of abuse have been reported to produce both rewarding and aversive effects, as evidenced by their ability to induce both conditioned place preferences (CPPs) and conditioned taste aversions (CTAs), respectively. Although several attempts have been made to assess the relationship between the rewarding and aversive effects of drugs in independent groups, it is unknown to what extent (if any) preferences and aversions are related in individual animals. The present study assessed this relationship by examini… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
31
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 47 publications
(36 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
5
31
0
Order By: Relevance
“…First, given the recently reported motor sensitizing effects of cathinone pre-exposure (see Berquist et al, 2016; Gregg et al, 2013; Watterson et al, 2015), such a history could also impact MDPV’s rewarding effects (e.g., changes in MDPV-induced place preference conditioning) and until these assessments are made it is impossible to predict the direction of change in the overall balance and how the change might impact the drug’s self-administration. This is especially relevant given that the aversive and rewarding effects of drugs appear to be dissociable, i.e., changes in one effect is not necessarily paralleled by changes in the other (see Simpson and Riley, 2005; Verendeev and Riley, 2011; for a review, see Verendeev and Riley, 2012). Secondly, independent assessments of changes in drug intake need to be directly made, i.e., the effects of drug history on MDPV self-administration must be assessed to determine the potential for increased and/or escalated use.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, given the recently reported motor sensitizing effects of cathinone pre-exposure (see Berquist et al, 2016; Gregg et al, 2013; Watterson et al, 2015), such a history could also impact MDPV’s rewarding effects (e.g., changes in MDPV-induced place preference conditioning) and until these assessments are made it is impossible to predict the direction of change in the overall balance and how the change might impact the drug’s self-administration. This is especially relevant given that the aversive and rewarding effects of drugs appear to be dissociable, i.e., changes in one effect is not necessarily paralleled by changes in the other (see Simpson and Riley, 2005; Verendeev and Riley, 2011; for a review, see Verendeev and Riley, 2012). Secondly, independent assessments of changes in drug intake need to be directly made, i.e., the effects of drug history on MDPV self-administration must be assessed to determine the potential for increased and/or escalated use.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…On the other hand, in non-feeding situations, the very same drug of abuse might be self-administered and its rewarding properties support the development of, for example, a conditioned place preference (e.g., Berger, 1972; Reicher & Holman, 1977; Wise, Yokel & DeWit, 1976). Furthermore, these two very different behavioral outcomes, a taste aversion and a place preference, can be produced concurrently by the same drug injection (Verendeev & Riley, 2011; Wang, Huang, & Hsiao, 2010). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This phenomenon, referred to as reward comparison, was described by Grigson (1997) and is thought to be similar to the anticipatory contrast effect in which rats avoid intake of the same saccharin cue when it predicts the opportunity to consume a highly rewarding sucrose solution [810]. In support, reward-sensitive Lewis rats are more sensitive to the suppressive effects of cocaine and sucrose, but not LiCl, than are less sensitive Fischer 344 rats [1113]. In Sprague-Dawley rats, a history of treatment with chronic morphine leads to greater avoidance of a taste cue paired with cocaine or sucrose, but not the aversive agent, LiCl [14].…”
Section: 1 Introductionmentioning
confidence: 90%