1996
DOI: 10.1901/jaba.1996.29-153
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Responding Maintained by Intermittent Reinforcement: Implications for the Use of Extinction With Problem Behavior in Clinical Settings

Abstract: Results of basic research have demonstrated that behavior maintained on an intermittent schedule of reinforcement (INT) will be extinguished more slowly than behavior maintained on a continuous schedule (CRF). Although these findings suggest that problem behaviors may be difficult to treat with extinction if they have been maintained on INT rather than on CRF schedules, few applied studies have examined this phenomenon with human behavior in clinical settings. The purpose of this study was to determine whether… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

2
33
0

Year Published

1996
1996
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 46 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
2
33
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition, reinforcement schedules may increase the likelihood or amount of other characteristics of responding, including resistance to extinction and disinhibition, and may alter the effects of reinforcement magnitude on resistance. Nevertheless, only a few applied studies have examined the PREE (e.g., R. Baer, Blount, Detrich, & Stokes, 1987;Kazdin & Polster, 1973;Koegel & Rincover, 1977), and each contains some potential limitations that prevent clear interpretation of the data (see Lerman, Iwata, Shore, & Kahng, 1996, for a detailed discussion of these problems).…”
Section: Applied Research and Implicationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In addition, reinforcement schedules may increase the likelihood or amount of other characteristics of responding, including resistance to extinction and disinhibition, and may alter the effects of reinforcement magnitude on resistance. Nevertheless, only a few applied studies have examined the PREE (e.g., R. Baer, Blount, Detrich, & Stokes, 1987;Kazdin & Polster, 1973;Koegel & Rincover, 1977), and each contains some potential limitations that prevent clear interpretation of the data (see Lerman, Iwata, Shore, & Kahng, 1996, for a detailed discussion of these problems).…”
Section: Applied Research and Implicationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A recent study by Lerman et al (1996) examined the PREE with severe behavior disorders using two different experimental designs and measures of resistance recommended by Nevin (1988). After sources of reinforcement that were maintaining 3 subjects' self-injury, aggression, or disruption were identified via functional analyses, the individuals were exposed to extinction following baseline conditions with CRF or PRF schedules alternated within reversal or multielement designs.…”
Section: Applied Research and Implicationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This influential paper not only aided practitioners by making reinforcer deliveries more practical, but also greatly aided the clients they served. Bijou's studies on patterns of reinforcement with children are undoubtedly amongst some of the greatest contributions to the field, as evidenced by current research on practices such as error correction (e.g., Worsdell et al, 2005), punishment of problem behavior (e.g., Lerman, Iwata, Shore, & DeLeon, 1997), extinction of problem behavior (e.g., Lerman, Iwata, Shore, & Kahng, 1996), functional communication training (e.g., Worsdell et al, 2000), and delayed reinforcement (e.g., Freeland & Noell, 1999), to name a few areas of research informed by Bijou's work.…”
Section: "Patterns Of Reinforcement and Extinction In Youngmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…) Point #9: MOs are separate from schedules of reinforcement A high rate of behavior can be generated by the manipulation of various schedules of reinforcement (Ferster & Skinner, 1957). For example, a variable ratio schedule of reinforcement for self-injury, aggression, or other 20 problem behaviors can generate strong and persistent patterns of behavior that may be difficult to reduce (e.g., Lerman, Iwata, Shore, & Kahng, 1996). Strong and persistent behavior can also be a function of powerful antecedent variables such as extreme deprivation, or painful aversive stimulation.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%