2004
DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.30.1.56
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Response Selection in Visual Search: The Influence of Response Compatibility of Nontargets.

Abstract: The authors used visual search tasks in which components of the classic flanker task (B. A. Eriksen & C. W. Eriksen, 1974) were introduced. In several experiments the authors obtained evidence of parallel search for a target among distractor elements. Therefore, 2-stage models of visual search predict no effect of the identity of those distractors. However, clear compatibility effects of the distractors were obtained: Responses were faster when the distractors were compatible with the response than when they w… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

5
15
0

Year Published

2005
2005
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 73 publications
5
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…(For related neuroimaging data, see Stewart et al, 2003.) Similar RT effects from incidental stimuli have been found in flanker paradigms (Starreveld, Theeuwes, & Mortier, 2004).…”
mentioning
confidence: 55%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…(For related neuroimaging data, see Stewart et al, 2003.) Similar RT effects from incidental stimuli have been found in flanker paradigms (Starreveld, Theeuwes, & Mortier, 2004).…”
mentioning
confidence: 55%
“…In traditional response-interference paradigms (e.g., flanker paradigms; Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974;Starreveld et al, 2004), distracters are in close proximity to targets (e.g., the letter H or an arrow) and are generally from the same class of stimuli as distracters (e.g., other letters or arrows). Unlike previous paradigms, the stimulus dimensions to be ignored in our experiment were irrelevant to the task at hand, making them more representative of the kinds of ambient stimuli (e.g., street signs, advertisements, tools, persons) that are of interest to the social psychologist.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…On the other hand, it is also possible that distractor line orientation did not directly bypass attention, but that it was modulated by attention. Line orientation compatibility effects are expected to be stronger when attention is deployed to the distractor, because allocating spatial attention to the distractor will lead to processing of the orientation of the gray nontarget line inside the distractor, whereas the line orientation inside the distractor will mostly be ignored when attention is not deployed to the distractor (e.g., Becker, 2007;Starreveld, Theeuwes, & Mortier, 2004;Theeuwes & Burger, 1998). The finding of stronger distractor line effects for relevant color distractors and for distractors with stronger spatial interference effects is thus not unexpected.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore, if the meaning of a distractor word in a search task influences performance when parallel search is evident (i.e., flat slope), this would provide strong evidence for the obligatory nature of visual word recognition. In contrast, if that same word does not influence performance when search is efficient, one would conclude that this attribute of the distractor (i.e., semantics) was not processed in parallel or available (e.g., Starreveld, Theeuwes, & Mortier, 2004 Visual word recognition is commonly argued to be automatic in the sense that it is obligatory and ballistic. The present experiments combined Stroop and visual search paradigms to provide a novel test of this claim.…”
Section: Visual Searchmentioning
confidence: 99%