1995
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00327.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Response Style and Cross-Cultural Comparisons of Rating Scales Among East Asian and North American Students

Abstract: This report examines cross-cultural differences in response style regarding the use of rating scales Subjects were high school students 944 from Sendai (Japan), 1,357 from Taipei (Taiwan), 687 from Edmonton and Calgary (Canada), and 2,174 from the Minneapolis metropolitan area and Fairfax County, Virginia Responses to fifty-seven 7-point Likert-type scales were analyzed The Japanese and Chinese students were more likely than the two North American groups to use the midpoint on the scales, the U S subjects were… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

18
458
9
4

Year Published

2005
2005
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
5
5

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 697 publications
(489 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
18
458
9
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Because the Chinese response style tends to be more moderate, rather than at the extremes of a scale (Chen, Lee, & Stevenson, 1995), it may be that CA parents actually do exert more control over their children. According to Ho (1996), the guiding principle of filial piety "justifies absolute parental authority over children" (p. 156).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because the Chinese response style tends to be more moderate, rather than at the extremes of a scale (Chen, Lee, & Stevenson, 1995), it may be that CA parents actually do exert more control over their children. According to Ho (1996), the guiding principle of filial piety "justifies absolute parental authority over children" (p. 156).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The possibility of cultural differences in response styles to Likert scales presents a potential artifactual phenomenon that could affect our findings. For example, respondents from more collectivist cultures may be less likely than those from individualistic cultures to use the extreme points in a scale (Chen et al, 1995). Thus standardization of survey item scores by country, prior to forming scale scores (e.g., a scale score for visionary leadership for a given firm in a given country), provides a means of controlling for such rating processes.…”
Section: Analysesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are many methodological challenges to comparing cultures, and cross-cultural comparisons of mean responses to subjective Likert scales is a method that is especially prone to methodological artifacts. Without proper experimental controls, such comparisons suffer from moderacy response biases (e.g., Chen, Lee, & Stevenson, 1995), acquiescent response styles (e.g., Choi & Choi, 2002), deprivation effects (Peng et al, 1997), and reference-group effects (Heine, Lehman, Peng, & Greenholtz, 2002). Because of these shortcomings that are inherent in cultural comparisons of Likert scale measures, which are used in so many applications of the cross-cultural survey, this method for exploring universals has rarely been used to its full potential.…”
Section: Cross-cultural Studymentioning
confidence: 99%