2016
DOI: 10.1152/jn.00574.2016
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Role of motor execution in the ocular tracking of self-generated movements

Abstract: When human observers track the movements of their own hand with their gaze, the eyes can start moving before the finger (i.e., anticipatory smooth pursuit). The signals driving anticipation could come from motor commands during finger motor execution or from motor intention and decision processes associated with self-initiated movements. For the present study, we built a mechanical device that could move a visual target either in the same direction as the participant's hand or in the opposite direction. Gaze p… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
11
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
2
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…2 B ). This task was designed to probe the ability to predict the visual consequences of one’s hand movement (Vercher et al, 1995; Chen et al, 2016; Danion et al, 2017). Participants were asked to generate random target movements so as to make target motion as unpredictable as possible (Steinbach and Held, 1968; Landelle et al, 2016; Mathew et al, 2017).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…2 B ). This task was designed to probe the ability to predict the visual consequences of one’s hand movement (Vercher et al, 1995; Chen et al, 2016; Danion et al, 2017). Participants were asked to generate random target movements so as to make target motion as unpredictable as possible (Steinbach and Held, 1968; Landelle et al, 2016; Mathew et al, 2017).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Interestingly, when participants were asked to observe two players throwing a ball back and forth instead of actively throwing and catching it themselves, predictive saccades were less accurate, and fewer periods of smooth pursuit were observed between bounce and catch (Hayhoe, Mennie, Sullivan, & Gorgos, 2005). Similarly, laboratory studies have shown that actively engaging in tasks such as manual tracking, catching, or pointing can enhance simultaneous pursuit eye movements (Chen, Valsecchi, & Gegenfurtner, 2016;Danion & Flanagan, 2018;Fooken, Lalonde, Mann, & Spering, 2018). Conversely, pursuing a ball seems to provide information about the ball's motion, which is important for predicting its trajectory (Bennett, Baures, Hecht, & Benguigui, 2010;Spering, Schütz, Braun, & Gegenfurtner, 2011) and for controlling the timing and accuracy of interceptive movements directed at the ball (Brenner & Smeets, 2011;de la Malla et al, 2016;de la Malla et al, 2017;Fooken, Yeo, Pai, & Spering, 2016;Leclercq, Blohm, & Lefèvre, 2013).…”
Section: How Predictive Signals Drive Eye Movements When Interacting mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In previous studies investigating this task, only the right dominant hand was used (Vercher et al, 1993, 1996; Scarchilli and Vercher, 1999; Chen et al, 2016a; Landelle et al, 2016; Danion et al, 2017; Mathew et al, 2017, 2018) or no (or incomplete) information was provided regarding participants’ handedness or the hand used in the task (Steinbach and Held, 1968; Steinbach, 1969; Gauthier and Hofferer, 1976; Gauthier et al, 1988). To date, we are only aware of a single study in which dominant and non-dominant hands were used (Chen et al, 2016b), but the putative impact of handedness was not reported.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%