2003
DOI: 10.1348/000712603762842156
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Seeking the middle ground in the ‘memory wars’

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
21
0
1

Year Published

2006
2006
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 30 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
0
21
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Specifically, despite the diversity in methodologies and results, it often remains unclear whether participants in false memory studies actually remember the suggested false event or merely believe they experienced it. Nevertheless, as we have argued before (Ost, 2003;Smeets et al, 2005), it is important to differentiate between belief and memory. For example, McNally, Clancy, Schacter, and Pitman (2000) found subtle differences between patients with recovered memories of abuse and patients with beliefs about childhood abuse in that patients who merely believed in that they had been abused scored higher on measures of absorption and dissociation compared to patients with recovered memories of abuse, who in turn scored higher than patients who had always remembered their abuse.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Specifically, despite the diversity in methodologies and results, it often remains unclear whether participants in false memory studies actually remember the suggested false event or merely believe they experienced it. Nevertheless, as we have argued before (Ost, 2003;Smeets et al, 2005), it is important to differentiate between belief and memory. For example, McNally, Clancy, Schacter, and Pitman (2000) found subtle differences between patients with recovered memories of abuse and patients with beliefs about childhood abuse in that patients who merely believed in that they had been abused scored higher on measures of absorption and dissociation compared to patients with recovered memories of abuse, who in turn scored higher than patients who had always remembered their abuse.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Some may be, some might not be. Some never "got" memories (Ost, 2003). As Bartlett (1932) suggested, reasoning, rather than conscious recollection, is important in inferring the "probable constituents" of one's past, and that socially situated beliefs (or "attitudes") then drive recollection.…”
Section: Conclusion and Wider Implicationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Ost (2003), drawing on the work of Sir Frederic Bartlett, argued for the primacy of "attitudes" (or beliefs) over recall (or recollection). Rather than directly recalling past events, Bartlett argued that people start by making inferences about their current circumstances based on what must have happened in their past ("This and this and this must have occurred in order that my present state should be what it is"; Bartlett, 1932, p. 202, emphasis added).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Based on this burgeoning literature, researchers (DePrince, Allard, Oh, & Freyd, 2004;Ost, 2003;Rubin, Schrauf, & Greenberg, 2003;Scoboria, Mazzoni, Kirsch, & Relyea, 2004;Smeets, Merckelbach, Horselenberg, & Jelicic, 2006) have made a distinction between the concepts of autobiographical belief (believing that an event occurred, whether or not one has an accompanying memory; e.g., most individuals believe they were born, but do not remember the event), and autobiographical memory (remembering the event, with accompanying perceptual detail). This distinction has important implications for understanding false autobiographical remembering.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%