2006
DOI: 10.3758/bf03193590
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Semantic priming over unrelated trials: Evidence for different effects in word and picture naming

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

4
27
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(31 citation statements)
references
References 65 publications
4
27
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For instance, Wheeldon and Monsell (1994) required participants to name pictures and written definitions of objects and observed that naming latencies to picture targets (e.g., “shark”) were slower when some trials previously a semantic coordinate word (e.g., “whale”) was produced as a response to a written definition, compared to when a non-semantic coordinate was previously produced (e.g., “volcano”). Similar semantic costs are reported if instead of naming written definitions, participants name objects (Vitkovitch et al, 2006). Furthermore, when several objects of the same semantic category are presented to be named, the amount of interference is cumulative, so that the amount of the delay observed for each additional instance of the category that is named depends on the total number of exemplars of the same category that have already been named (Brown, 1981; Howard et al, 2006; Costa et al, 2009; Navarrete et al, 2010).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 64%
“…For instance, Wheeldon and Monsell (1994) required participants to name pictures and written definitions of objects and observed that naming latencies to picture targets (e.g., “shark”) were slower when some trials previously a semantic coordinate word (e.g., “whale”) was produced as a response to a written definition, compared to when a non-semantic coordinate was previously produced (e.g., “volcano”). Similar semantic costs are reported if instead of naming written definitions, participants name objects (Vitkovitch et al, 2006). Furthermore, when several objects of the same semantic category are presented to be named, the amount of interference is cumulative, so that the amount of the delay observed for each additional instance of the category that is named depends on the total number of exemplars of the same category that have already been named (Brown, 1981; Howard et al, 2006; Costa et al, 2009; Navarrete et al, 2010).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 64%
“…In this lag2 condition, the time interval between the onset of the prime and target was 6 sec., similar to previous studies reporting semantic interference (e.g., Wheeldon & Monsell, 1994). Third, to make the primetarget pairs less obvious to participants, we followed Wheeldon and Monsell (1994) and Vitkovitch et al (2001, 2006), presenting pictures at a consistent rate (2 sec. ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Consistent with this hypothesis, the time interval between prime and target onsets is shorter in studies demonstrating facilitation (< 2 sec., Biggs & Marmurek, 1990; Huttenlocher & Kubicek, 1983; Lupker 1988; Sperber et al, 1979) compared to those demonstrating interference (> 4 sec., Vitkovitch et al, 2001; 2006; Wheeldon & Monsell, 1994). However, to allow for this conclusion, another explanation needs to be ruled out.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Similarly, response times for each subsequent within-category ordinal position encountered in the list are averaged across categories. Brown (1981) and later Howard and colleagues (2006) found that picture naming latencies increase with each within-category item that participants name—a phenomenon referred to as the ‘cumulative semantic cost’ (for related work, see Wheeldon & Monsell, 1994; Tree & Hirsh, 2003; Vitkovitch, Cooper-Pye, & Leadbetter, 2006; Vitkovitch, Cooper-Pye, & Ali, 2010; Vitkovitch, & Cooper-Pye, 2012). The Cumulative Semantic Cost is linear at least out to five ordinal positions within category (e.g., R 2 = .96 in Navarrete, Mahon, & Caramazza, 2010; see also Alario, & Moscoso del Prado Martin, 2010; Costa, Strijkers, Martin, & Thierry, 2009; Howard et al, 2006; Belke, 2013; Belke & Stielow, in press; Oppenheim, Dell, & Schwartz, 2010; Runnqvist, Strijkers, Alario, & Costa, 2012).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%