2007
DOI: 10.1002/ab.20207
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Sequence and priming in 15 month‐olds' reactions to brief arm restraint: evidence for a hierarchy of anger responses

Abstract: Brief, gentle arm restraint is widely used in experimental studies of children's anger, but the pattern of responses generated by such restraint has been incompletely described. We now describe a hierarchy of responses within trials as well as an escalation across trials that have both methodological and theoretical significance. Mothers of 87 15-month olds prevented them from playing with a toy by restraining their arms on two consecutive 30 sec trials. Physical struggling was the first and most frequent resp… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
4
1

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…First offenders cannot be completely certain of victory, and the behavioral and metabolic costs of fighting are high. Priming effects of aggressive conflicts with longer lasting consequences for the offender have been observed in hamsters, rats, and humans (Hebert, Potegal, & Meyerhoff, 1994; Potegal, 1992; Potegal, Robison, Anderson, Jordan, & Shapiro, 2007). The data here presented suggest that an MR-mediated signal of corticosterone during a first conflict probably initializes a neuronal circuit enabling a long-lasting facilitation of aggression in subsequent conflicts (see Figures 3 and 5).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First offenders cannot be completely certain of victory, and the behavioral and metabolic costs of fighting are high. Priming effects of aggressive conflicts with longer lasting consequences for the offender have been observed in hamsters, rats, and humans (Hebert, Potegal, & Meyerhoff, 1994; Potegal, 1992; Potegal, Robison, Anderson, Jordan, & Shapiro, 2007). The data here presented suggest that an MR-mediated signal of corticosterone during a first conflict probably initializes a neuronal circuit enabling a long-lasting facilitation of aggression in subsequent conflicts (see Figures 3 and 5).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To assess child anger in a context‐relevant manner, the current study used the Arm Restraint procedure (Goldsmith & Rothbart, 1996; Stenberg & Campos, 1990), an ecologically and cross‐culturally valid measure for evoking anger in infants (Bennett, Bendersky, & Lewis, 2002; Buss & Goldsmith, 1998; Camras, Oster, Campos, Miyake, & Bradshaw, 1992; Camras et al., 1998; Moscardino & Axia, 2006; Potegal, Robison, Anderson, Jordan, & Shapiro, 2007; Stenberg & Campos, 1990; Stifter & Spinrad, 2002). We expected to see an increase in frequency of anger in response to restrictions of movement and removal of toy when infants were seated in a chair because the infants’ goals of playing with a toy and of moving their arms freely were blocked.…”
Section: Crawling Experience and Anger Expressionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Behaviorally, tantrums are comprised of rapid-rising, intense anger followed by less intense, slower-fading distress. The anger component of tantrums has an organized, hierarchical pattern of increased activation and escalating vocal behavior to blocked play, empirically observable by 15 months of age (Potegal, Robison, Anderson, Jordan, & Shapiro, 2007). The organization of tantrums as a dynamic, bi-phasic process suggests that the initial anger of tantrums may be goal-focused.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%