2021
DOI: 10.1007/s12078-021-09290-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Sequential Effects in Odor Perception

Abstract: Introduction Our visual, auditory, and tactile perception is not solely determined by what we process at a given moment in time, but also determined by what we perceived recently. These sequential effects can either be negative (repulsive) or positive (assimilative). Here, we investigated whether such sequential effects also occur in odor perception. Method Participants rated a sequence of 40 different odors (varying widely in hedonic valence) on four different characteristics (intensity, familiarity, valence,… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 75 publications
(78 reference statements)
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These effects were most intensively studied in visual perception, where it was demonstrated for a broad range of simple (e.g., orientation [9][10][11] ; position 12,13 ) and complex features (e.g., facial identity 14,15 , facial gender and expression 16,17 , or facial attractiveness 18 ). Serial effects were also found in other modalities, including olfactory 19 and auditory stimuli [20][21][22][23][24] .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…These effects were most intensively studied in visual perception, where it was demonstrated for a broad range of simple (e.g., orientation [9][10][11] ; position 12,13 ) and complex features (e.g., facial identity 14,15 , facial gender and expression 16,17 , or facial attractiveness 18 ). Serial effects were also found in other modalities, including olfactory 19 and auditory stimuli [20][21][22][23][24] .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…The EmojiGrid was inspired by Russell’s Affect Grid [ 29 ] and was originally developed and validated for the affective appraisal of food stimuli [ 11 , 20 ], since conventional affective self-report tools (e.g., Self-Assessment Manikin [ 43 ]) are frequently misunderstood in that context [ 11 , 20 ]. It has since also successfully been used and validated for the affective appraisal of a wide range of different emotional stimuli, such as images [ 44 ], sound and video clips [ 45 ], touch events [ 46 ], odors [ 47 , 48 , 49 ] and VR experiences [ 50 ]. Since it is intuitive and language-independent, the EmojiGrid is also suitable for cross-cultural research [ 11 , 51 ] and research involving children or low-literate participants.…”
Section: Methods and Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While syllables can be presented hundreds, or even thousands, of times in an experimental session to collect sufficient data, this is simply much more difficult to achieve in the case of flavour stimuli. Note here also how participants adapt rapidly to olfactory or gustatory stimulation, and thus require much longer intervals and/or neutralizing stimuli to be presented between trials (see also [91,178]). Flavour stimuli are also more challenging to control precisely; for instance, asking participants to chew for a specified interval will result in higher variability than presenting video stimuli that can simply be repeated and counterbalanced across an experimental session.…”
Section: Practical Challengesmentioning
confidence: 99%